首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   20622篇
  免费   865篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2023年   104篇
  2021年   172篇
  2020年   318篇
  2019年   380篇
  2018年   507篇
  2017年   529篇
  2016年   541篇
  2015年   421篇
  2014年   519篇
  2013年   2371篇
  2012年   877篇
  2011年   920篇
  2010年   562篇
  2009年   620篇
  2008年   847篇
  2007年   838篇
  2006年   823篇
  2005年   778篇
  2004年   722篇
  2003年   728篇
  2002年   711篇
  2001年   349篇
  2000年   317篇
  1999年   328篇
  1998年   319篇
  1997年   295篇
  1996年   273篇
  1995年   261篇
  1994年   234篇
  1993年   223篇
  1992年   209篇
  1991年   221篇
  1990年   186篇
  1989年   181篇
  1988年   148篇
  1987年   167篇
  1986年   182篇
  1985年   230篇
  1984年   232篇
  1983年   205篇
  1982年   189篇
  1981年   204篇
  1980年   185篇
  1979年   147篇
  1978年   179篇
  1977年   165篇
  1976年   169篇
  1975年   151篇
  1974年   155篇
  1973年   127篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Behavioural evidence concerning short-sightedness in rats is apparently conflicting: in some experiments rats have performed poorly with visual stimuli further than about 60 cm distant, while in others they have made efficient use of more distant cues, for example to find their way through mazes. However, in the experiments suggesting short-sightedness, the physical size of the stimuli was not varied, so that stimulus distance and visual angle were confounded. In the present experiment, therefore, the size and distance of the stimuli to be detected were varied independently. Over the range tested (30-160 cm), distance was found to produce relatively slight effects on the smallest detectable visual angle, and these tended to diminish with practice. Thus, no good evidence was found for short-sightedness in rats up to 160 cm, a finding consistent with current views of the structure and image-forming capacities of the rat's eye. The smallest detectable targets were, however, surprisingly large in view of the rat's visual acuity (which is about 1c/deg): at the distances tested, animals required considerable training to run reliably to targets subtending less than 5-10° of visual angle. Difficulties in responding to stationary stimuli of this size are likely to restrict severely the use that rats make of vision both in the laboratory and in their natural surroundings.  相似文献   
22.
23.
24.
The authors used recursive partitioning methods to identify combinations of baseline characteristics that predict 2-year physical activity success in each of 3 physical activity interventions delivered in the multisite Activity Counseling Trial. The sample consisted of 874 initially sedentary primary care patients, ages 35-75 years, who were at risk for cardiovascular disease. Predictors of 2-year success were specific to each intervention and represented a range of domains, including physiological, demographic, psychosocial, health-related, and environmental variables. The results indicate how specific patient subgroups (e.g., obese, unfit individuals; high-income individuals in stable health) may respond differently to varying levels and amounts of professional assistance and support. The methods used provide a practical first step toward identifying clinically meaningful patient subgroups for further systematic investigation.  相似文献   
25.
26.
27.
Godden  David 《Topoi》2019,38(4):725-750
Topoi - Robert Fogelin argued that the efficacy of our acts of reasons-giving depends on the normalcy of our discourse—to the extent that discourse is not normal disagreements occurring in it...  相似文献   
28.
Tibbetts  Paul 《Synthese》2004,141(2):247-276
Synthese - The concept of voluntary motor control(VMC) frequently appears in the neuroscientific literature, specifically in the context of cortically-mediated, intentional motor actions. For...  相似文献   
29.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号