全文获取类型
收费全文 | 13732篇 |
免费 | 583篇 |
国内免费 | 13篇 |
专业分类
14328篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 72篇 |
2021年 | 117篇 |
2020年 | 213篇 |
2019年 | 262篇 |
2018年 | 358篇 |
2017年 | 352篇 |
2016年 | 372篇 |
2015年 | 267篇 |
2014年 | 345篇 |
2013年 | 1531篇 |
2012年 | 609篇 |
2011年 | 652篇 |
2010年 | 390篇 |
2009年 | 419篇 |
2008年 | 566篇 |
2007年 | 563篇 |
2006年 | 571篇 |
2005年 | 521篇 |
2004年 | 479篇 |
2003年 | 504篇 |
2002年 | 487篇 |
2001年 | 244篇 |
2000年 | 221篇 |
1999年 | 217篇 |
1998年 | 199篇 |
1997年 | 213篇 |
1996年 | 192篇 |
1995年 | 169篇 |
1994年 | 158篇 |
1993年 | 153篇 |
1992年 | 140篇 |
1991年 | 152篇 |
1990年 | 135篇 |
1989年 | 120篇 |
1988年 | 88篇 |
1987年 | 117篇 |
1986年 | 127篇 |
1985年 | 162篇 |
1984年 | 160篇 |
1983年 | 138篇 |
1982年 | 130篇 |
1981年 | 143篇 |
1980年 | 124篇 |
1979年 | 108篇 |
1978年 | 118篇 |
1977年 | 108篇 |
1976年 | 107篇 |
1975年 | 94篇 |
1974年 | 104篇 |
1973年 | 85篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
David Ingle 《Psychological research》1967,31(1):44-51
Summary Studies of motion-detection by fish reveal two separate processes, which are hypothetically linked to different kinds of behavior. Furthermore, studies of interocular transfer with mirror-image shape discriminations also indicate two distinct mechanisms for the representation of visual directions. From these two kinds of experiment, it seems that visual processes subserving orientation of the fish to a moving object should be clearly distinguished from processes by which the fish evaluates the identity or activity of the object.
Zusammenfassung Untersuchungen der Bewegungswahrnehmung von Fischen zeigen zwei verschiedene Prozesse auf, die mit zwei verschiedenen Verhaltensweisen in Beziehung gesetzt werden. Eine Analyse des interokularen Transfers mit Unterscheidung spiegelbildlicher Formen läßt außerdem erkennen, daß zwei verschiedene Mechanismen für die Repräsentation visueller Richtungen verantwortlich sind. Aus diesen Experimenten wird geschlossen, daß jene visuellen Prozesse, welche der Orientierung des Fisches in bezug auf ein sich bewegendes Objekt dienen, scharf unterschieden werden sollten von denen, durch welche der Fisch die Identität oder Aktivität des Objektes beurteilt.相似文献
132.
Modifications of the intake procedure usually employed in counseling settings were adopted in order to provide students the opportunity for immediate contact with a counselor. Elimination of the waiting list, reduction in number of missed appointments, and reduction of length of counseling came about. Staff satisfaction with the new approach has been high and there would be opposition to reverting to the scheduled initial interview. Possible changes in the type of problem or clients' readiness for problem-solving were observed. 相似文献
133.
David Krausman 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1967,2(2):173-176
This paper describes a miniature solid-state timing device particularly useful for presenting repetitive signal sequences in classical conditioning experiments. The device uses transistors, silicon-controlled rectifiers, and four-layer diodes, and is powered by 20-hour rechargeable nickel-cadmium batteries. Visual monitoring is provided for the unelapsed ITI and battery charge. Ranges are; ITI, 15 sec. to 5 min., ISI, 0–50 secs., CS duration, 0.5 to 30 sec., US duration, 0.5 to 30 sec. The CS and US durations are adjustable and the onset of the latter may be at any time after the CS onset. Cycling is continuous and automatic; the outputs are in the form of relay closures. 相似文献
134.
135.
David R. Saunders 《Psychometrika》1960,25(2):199-205
A modification of the quartimax computation for factor rotation is described in which a hypothesized factor pattern is given to the machine along with the data. The machine uses the pattern to select the subset of variables to which it will attend when rotating in a given plane, in order to find an orthogonal solution which closely fits the hypothesis. The program also provides a measure of the goodness of this fit. The program can utilize pattern matrices that reflect only partial hypotheses as to the nature of the factors, as well as those that specify highly determined simple structure. 相似文献
136.
David Rynin 《Synthese》1960,12(1):6-24
Conclusion We can now see, perhaps, what merits if any this definition has. It ties in the concept of evidence with that of law, and requires us to support our claim that some fact (or statement) is evidence for another by pointing out the science, and within it the law (or what meets the tests of being acceptable as such) whose existence alone justifies us in assuming that a certain conditional statement qualifies as acceptable as nomological. Whoever claims that a certain fact is evidence for another is required to justify his assumption that there exists a certain law. That in some sense there are such justifiable assumptions is clear from the existence of natural sciences and technologies based upon them. But it will turn out that in many cases it is impossible, as of a given moment, to make out a case for the claim that a certain conditional statement, required to support the view that a certain fact is evidence for another, is nomological. In such cases we are able to draw a clear line between what someone may think or claim is evidence, and what we have reasonable grounds for accepting as such. In short the concept offers us a principle of demarcation between justified and unjustified claims for the evidential nature of facts (or statements) offered as such. If this is not a very impressive achievement still it is not wholly contemptible, considering the confusion that prevails among many people concerning the question of evidence. 相似文献
137.
138.
David D. Clark 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1966,44(8):817-823
Factual and subjective data gathered from counseling centers in 36 major universities is presented to show present conditions, emphasis, and needs of the future. Data are presented showing ratios of counselors to students, budget allotments, strengths, specific weaknesses, etc. Counseling facilities, services provided, emphasis, communications and plans for expansion are dealt with individually. Less than half the centers were meeting student needs and a majority indicated a shortage of professional staff, particularly those competent to do personal adjustment counseling. 相似文献
139.
David V. Budescu Thomas S. Wallsten 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》1990,46(2)
The goal of the research was to compare decisions under risk in a situation in which forecasters (F) communicate to decision makers (DM) either numerically (e.g., .70) or verbally (e.g., likely) about the chances that a binary event will occur. Following each forecast, the DM bid for a winning or losing lottery based on the event. In Experiment 1 Fs and DMs also provided numerical translations of each verbal forecast after the DMs' bid. In Experiment 2 the DMs provided membership functions over the [0, 1] interval for each phrase used by the Fs. The primary results were: (a) extreme similarity in the DM's bids and rates of bidding under the two modes of communication; (b) greater variability in bids to specific verbal than numerical forecasts; (c) a pattern of bids, in which DMs demonstrated risk seeking for gains and risk neutrality for losses; (d) DMs' numerical translations in Experiment 1 were closer to .50 than were those of Fs; and (e) phrases selected by Fs had high membership values to DMs for the probabilities the Fs were attempting to describe. Points (a), (b), (d), and (e) are consistent with the ν-μ model which assumes that the vague meaning of a probability phrase can be represented by a membership function over the [0, 1] interval, and that in reaching a decision the DM focuses on a range of probabilities with sufficiently high membership. Point (c) is speculatively attributed to social aspects of the dyadic situation, and requires further investigation. 相似文献
140.