全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39907篇 |
免费 | 698篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
40610篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 453篇 |
2019年 | 509篇 |
2018年 | 639篇 |
2017年 | 676篇 |
2016年 | 746篇 |
2015年 | 566篇 |
2014年 | 676篇 |
2013年 | 3314篇 |
2012年 | 1210篇 |
2011年 | 1273篇 |
2010年 | 804篇 |
2009年 | 828篇 |
2008年 | 1140篇 |
2007年 | 1094篇 |
2006年 | 1027篇 |
2005年 | 965篇 |
2004年 | 902篇 |
2003年 | 931篇 |
2002年 | 945篇 |
2001年 | 927篇 |
2000年 | 892篇 |
1999年 | 678篇 |
1998年 | 432篇 |
1997年 | 434篇 |
1996年 | 396篇 |
1992年 | 599篇 |
1991年 | 614篇 |
1990年 | 590篇 |
1989年 | 567篇 |
1988年 | 494篇 |
1987年 | 532篇 |
1986年 | 569篇 |
1985年 | 637篇 |
1984年 | 556篇 |
1983年 | 505篇 |
1982年 | 398篇 |
1981年 | 457篇 |
1979年 | 540篇 |
1978年 | 428篇 |
1977年 | 391篇 |
1975年 | 497篇 |
1974年 | 560篇 |
1973年 | 562篇 |
1972年 | 459篇 |
1971年 | 449篇 |
1970年 | 414篇 |
1969年 | 434篇 |
1968年 | 517篇 |
1967年 | 501篇 |
1966年 | 479篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
William D Siegfried Iain Macfarlane David B Graham Nil A Moore Patricia L Young 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1981,18(1):30-42
Previous research has found conflicting answers to the question of whether males and females share similar preferences for various job characteristics. It is difficult to determine if the different findings are due to the different populations sampled, the various methodologies employed, or to a real change in the phenomenon itself. This study essentially replicates earlier work by having 130 male and 84 female college students rate the importance of 10 job characteristics for both themselves and members of the opposite sex. Both sexes rated motivators as important, but females also placed importance on the hygienes. Males tended to underestimate the importance of the motivators for females, while females overestimated the importance of hygienes for males. The subject's sex could be predicted by both the importance for self and importance for opposite sex ratings, contrary to the earlier study. The difference in results and interpretation was shown to be due to the type of analyses used. Additionally, it was found that females' job preferences were related to their mothers' educational achievement. 相似文献
902.
In 14 experiments, subjects had to “point to” surrounding environmental locations (targets) while imagining themselves in a particular spot facing in various directions (orientations). The spatial information was either committed to memory (cognitive maps) or directly presented on each trial in the visual or tactile modality. Reaction times (RT) indicated that orientation shifts were achieved through mental rotation in the visual task, but not in the cognitive map or tactile tasks. Further, in the latter two tasks targets were located most quickly when they were adjacent to or opposite the imagined orientation. Several explanations of this finding were tested. Various aspects of the data suggest that cognitive maps are not strictly holistic, but consist of orientation-specific representations, and—at least in part—of relational propositions specific to object pairs. 相似文献
903.
David J Hargreaves Philip M Jones Diane Martin 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1981,32(1):11-20
The “air gap” phenomenon was found to be very common in the free landscape drawings of middle and later childhood. Three experiments are reported in which the contextual cues for such drawings were systematically manipulated. When subjects were supplied with drawing sheets constructed to imply the absence of the air gap, the phenomenon was eliminated in most of their drawings. When supplied with sheets constructed to imply its presence, most subjects produced air gap drawings using contextually provided reference lines. There were no consistent transfer effects across different cuing orders. The susceptibility of the air gap phenomenon to modification by task manipulation suggests that it results from problems of production rather than from defects in the child's internal representation of the world. 相似文献
904.
905.
FRANCIS H. NORTON 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1981,60(3):156-159
This article combines a foster parent's and an educator's view of working with children in foster care placements. Counseling a child who has been removed from the home environment requires a real sensitivity to the internal devastation that may be taking place and the coping behaviors associated with that separation process. 相似文献
906.
T H Nilsson T M Nelson 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1981,7(1):141-150
Short-term memory for 16 monochromatic hues from 425 to 640 nm was measured after six delays from .1 to 24.3 sec by means of an iterative, momentary stimulus-matching technique. Small shifts were revealed in the remembered hue produced by certain wavelengths at some delays. These shifts did not follow trends consistent with a storage dependent on sensory pathway characteristics, perceptually unique hues, or semantic encoding but may reflect entropic effects in a storage that is remarkably unbiased. By indicating the discriminability of hues in memory, standard deviations of the delayed matches reveal other characteristics of what is stored: Their smooth, exponential growth questions the existence of "levels" and permits estimating the half-life of hue memory; their continued resemblance to the discrimination function for simultaneously perceived hues suggests that the stored activity; closely resembled the sensory response of color. The results also indicate how successive comparisons may be corrected in applied color work. 相似文献
907.
H Poizner U Bellugi V Lutes-Driscoll 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1981,7(2):430-440
Perception of dynamic events of American Sign Language (ASL) was studied by isolating information about motion in the language from information about form. Four experiments utilized Johansson's technique for presenting biological motion as moving points of light. In the first, deaf signers were highly accurate in matching movements of lexical signs presented in point-light displays to those normally presented. Both discrimination accuracy and the pattern of errors were similar in this matching task to that obtained in a control condition in which the same signs were always represented normally. The second experiment showed that these results held for discrimination of morphological operations presented in point-light displays as well. In the third experiment, signers were able to accurately identify signs of a constant handshape and morphological operations acting on signs presented in point-light displays. Finally, in Experiment 4, we evaluated what aspects of the motion patterns carried most of the information for sign identifiability. We presented signs in point-light displays with certain lights removed and found that the movement of the fingertips, but not of any other pair of points, is necessary for sign identification and that, in general, the more distal the joint, the more information its movement carries. 相似文献
908.
G Rabending R Beier R Cammann H J Du Chesne H Gebelt M Grimmberger D Hobusch H Klepel K K?nig H H Knaape B Michalowski D Müller B Oettinger G Suhr H Todt J Vesper U Wendt H Zettler 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1981,33(10):618-628
The testing of Convulsofin carried out in 220 patients at 14 clinics over a period of six months confirms the international experience gained with valproic acid and sodium valproinate. The main field of application of Convulsofin according to the experience gained will again be the treatment of fits in generalised primary epilepsy for which it permits to carry out a monotherapy to a remarkable extent. The favourable effect of the preparation in photosensitivity could be confirmed. Also in generalised secondary epilepsy and in fits of partial epilepsy. Convulsofin is partly effective, so that it can recommended as a co-medication in the treatment of therapy-resistant forms of these epilepsies which are difficult to treat. Decrease in thrombocytes, transient increase in serum transaminases, gastrointestinal disorders, loss of hair and undesired increases in body-weight were the more frequently occurring side-effects. 相似文献
909.
In order to assess the role of the structural components of the Mueller-Lyer illusion, subjects reproduced the central extent of standard Mueller-Lyer figures and configural variations. Illusory magnitude of the underestimated wings-in and overestimated wings-out figures was examined with selective amputations of the oblique wings and central line segment (shaft). Variations were presented at 0, 45, 90, and 135 deg from vertical. Orientation had no reliable effect on illusory magnitude. Elimination of the shaft effected a decrease in the apparent extent for all variations, presumably due to the addition of the filled-unfilled space illusory effect to the standard Mueller-Lyer effect. A second study corroborated this finding The decrease in apparent extent consequent to shaft removal occurred independently of any response factor. Selective wing removal differentially decreased the illusory magnitude of the standard Mueller-Lyer figures; this was discussed with regard to a dual-illusion hypothesis. Finally, variations that contained no intersecting lines produced a significant illusion in the direction of the standard Mueller-Lyer figures, suggesting the involvement of higher level, nonperipheral distortion mechanisms. 相似文献
910.