首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17072篇
  免费   695篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2021年   134篇
  2020年   238篇
  2019年   306篇
  2018年   410篇
  2017年   404篇
  2016年   438篇
  2015年   312篇
  2014年   399篇
  2013年   1877篇
  2012年   699篇
  2011年   763篇
  2010年   463篇
  2009年   492篇
  2008年   686篇
  2007年   663篇
  2006年   676篇
  2005年   623篇
  2004年   564篇
  2003年   607篇
  2002年   600篇
  2001年   297篇
  2000年   264篇
  1999年   272篇
  1998年   236篇
  1997年   256篇
  1996年   235篇
  1995年   202篇
  1994年   202篇
  1993年   205篇
  1992年   176篇
  1991年   204篇
  1990年   177篇
  1989年   151篇
  1988年   127篇
  1987年   160篇
  1986年   160篇
  1985年   199篇
  1984年   218篇
  1983年   180篇
  1982年   184篇
  1981年   201篇
  1980年   163篇
  1979年   155篇
  1978年   156篇
  1977年   166篇
  1976年   159篇
  1975年   144篇
  1974年   157篇
  1973年   136篇
  1972年   83篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 343 毫秒
71.
The Treatment Evaluation Inventory (TEI), a frequently used measure of treatment acceptability, was used by 164 undergraduates to rate the acceptability of each of the following treatments: differential reinforcement of other behavior, exclusionary time-out, overcorrection, medical restraint, contingent electric shock, and physical restraint. TEI ratings of each treatment type were grouped separately, variance-covariance matrices were formed and compared, and data were subjected to factor analysis. The results indicated that the factor structure of the TEI varied with the treatment it was used to evaluate. Item analysis of the TEI indicated a high degree of internal consistency, although item-total correlations varied between rated treatments. The findings suggest that although the TEI is a reliable instrument, sensitive assessment of the treatment acceptability construct probably requires multidimensional measurement.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
The present research examined whether line-ups based on target (‘suspect’) face similarity are biased or suggestive. Four experiments are described in which subjects constructed photographic line-ups by selecting foils similar in appearance to a target. Later, another group of subjects who had not seen the faces before (mock witnesses) were asked to pick out the targets from the line-ups. All four experiments showed that mock witnesses selected the target significantly more often than expected by chance, thereby demonstrating suggestiveness. Three alternative line-up construction methods were also evaluated. In these methods, foil selection was based not only on target similarity but also on similarity with one or more of the other line-up faces. Results showed that alternative line-up targets were not selected significantly more often than chance, suggesting that bias was reduced. An overall analysis showed that the alternative line-ups were significantly less suggestive than target-based line-ups. The results indicate that foil selection procedures that incorporate foil-to-foil similarity produce fairer line-ups than those exclusively based on target similarity.  相似文献   
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
Neural networks can be used as a tool in the explanation of neuropsychological data. Using the Hebbian Learning Rule and other such principles as competition and modifiable interlevel feedback, researchers have successfully modeled a widely used neuropsychological test, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. One of these models is reviewed here and extended to a qualitative analysis of how verbal fluency might be modeled, which demonstrates the importance of accounting for the attentional components of both tests. Difficulties remain in programming sequential cognitive processes within a parallel distributed processing (PDP) framework and integrating exceedingly complex neuropsychological tests such as Proverbs. PDP neural network methodology offers neuropsychologists co-validation procedures within narrowly defined areas of reliability and validity.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号