全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17236篇 |
免费 | 265篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
17503篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 116篇 |
2022年 | 119篇 |
2021年 | 202篇 |
2020年 | 329篇 |
2019年 | 364篇 |
2018年 | 518篇 |
2017年 | 516篇 |
2016年 | 508篇 |
2015年 | 349篇 |
2014年 | 454篇 |
2013年 | 1910篇 |
2012年 | 828篇 |
2011年 | 872篇 |
2010年 | 542篇 |
2009年 | 492篇 |
2008年 | 751篇 |
2007年 | 700篇 |
2006年 | 689篇 |
2005年 | 586篇 |
2004年 | 538篇 |
2003年 | 573篇 |
2002年 | 530篇 |
2001年 | 271篇 |
2000年 | 257篇 |
1999年 | 237篇 |
1998年 | 217篇 |
1997年 | 227篇 |
1996年 | 202篇 |
1995年 | 176篇 |
1994年 | 168篇 |
1993年 | 165篇 |
1992年 | 147篇 |
1991年 | 169篇 |
1990年 | 143篇 |
1989年 | 130篇 |
1988年 | 97篇 |
1987年 | 126篇 |
1986年 | 134篇 |
1985年 | 175篇 |
1984年 | 170篇 |
1983年 | 142篇 |
1982年 | 135篇 |
1981年 | 149篇 |
1980年 | 129篇 |
1979年 | 113篇 |
1978年 | 120篇 |
1977年 | 110篇 |
1976年 | 108篇 |
1975年 | 98篇 |
1974年 | 107篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Previous literature has argued that proficient bilingual speakers often demonstrate monolingual-equivalent structural processing of language (e.g., the processing of structural ambiguities; Frenck-Mestre, 2002). In this paper, we explore this thesis further via on-line examination of the processing of syntactically complex structures with three populations: those who classify as monolingual native English speaker (MNES), those who classify as non-native English speakers (NNES), and those who calssify as bilingual native English speakers (BNES). On-line measures of processing of object-relative constructions demonstrated that both NNES and BNES have different patterns of performance as compared to MNES. Further, NNES and BNES speakers perform differently from one another in such processing. The study also examines the activation of lexical information in biasing contexts, and suggests that different processes are at work in the different type of bilinguals examined here. The nature of these differences and the implications for developing sensitive models of on-line language comprehension are developed and discussed. 相似文献
952.
Lester D 《Perceptual and motor skills》2003,96(2):370
In a sample of 61 psychology students, unity in the subjective structure of the self was associated with having a Taoist orientation. 相似文献
953.
Giménez de la Peña A Canto Ortiz JM Fernández Berrocal P Barrett M 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2003,6(1):28-34
Social psychologists have shown a profound interest in intergroup relationships, but there are very few papers focusing on the developmental aspects that explain the psychological mechanisms involved in the construction of group and cultural identity. Our research aims to explore how the self-categorization of Andalusian children evolves. We tried to assess the degree to which they self-identify as Andalusian, Spanish, and European, and how this identification changes with age. We were also interested in the affective evaluation of different groups (French, Italian, English, German, Spanish, Catalonian, and Andalusian) made by Andalusian children. Furthermore, we analyzed the relationship between self-categorization and the evaluation of these groups. Results show that the development of national (autonomous community) identity in these children is influenced by their cognitive development, as well as by the relationships among the regional communities of Spain and the relationships between Spain and other countries. The peculiarity of Andalusians as a group is that they assume both identities: Spanish and Andalusian, from a very early age. In-group favoritism is an extended phenomenon at all ages, and Andalusian children have a negative stereotype of the other Spanish groups and other European communities. 相似文献
954.
In this research we examined whether emotional vulnerability leads women and men to confirm gender stereotypes. Emotional vulnerability is a state where one is open to having one's feelings hurt or to experiencing rejection. Drawing on the tenets of social role theory and research related to normative expectations, we propose that emotional vulnerability leads to stereotype confirmation, as normative expectations are less risky and easier to enact than nonnormative behavior. Fifty-nine dating couples were randomly assigned to a high emotional vulnerability or low emotional vulnerability discussion with their partners. When the degree of emotional vulnerability was high men confirmed gender-stereotypes. Women's behavior, on the other hand, was not significantly affected by condition. We discuss these findings in terms of the domain in which gender-typed behaviors occur and the social pressures to act in accordance with gender norms. 相似文献
955.
956.
957.
Bellissimo MI Kouzmine I Ferro MM de Oliveira BH Canteras NS Da Cunha C 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2004,82(2):150-158
Adult male Wistar rats with a substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) lesion induced by intranigral administration of 1 micromol 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) were used as a model of early phase Parkinson's disease (PD). This lesion caused a partial depletion of striatal dopamine (DA). The animals were submitted to a spatial working memory version of the water maze task in which they had to find a hidden (submersed) platform using online-maintained information that the platform remains in the same place during four consecutive trials, but that it is moved to another place every training day. Left, but not right SNc-lesioned rats were impaired in finding the platform in the second trial. This result suggests that the left SNc plays a key role in spatial working memory. Control experiments ruled out the possibility that motor impairment, sensory neglect, and/or impairment in the mental representation of the contralateral spatial environment had affected performance of the SNc-lesioned rats. 相似文献
958.
Does intentional car following capture visual attention to the extent that driving may be impaired? We tested fifteen participants on a rudimentary driving simulator. Participants were either instructed to follow a vehicle ahead through a simulated version of London, or were given verbal instructions on where to turn during the route. The presence or absence of pedestrians, and the simulated time of the drive (day or night) were varied across the trials. Eye movements were recorded along with behavioural measures including give-way violations, give-way accidents, and kerb impacts. The results revealed that intentional car following reduced the spread of search and increased fixation durations, with a dramatic increase in the time spent processing the vehicle ahead (controlled for exposure). The effects were most pronounced during nighttime drives. During the car-following trials participants were also less aware of pedestrians, produced more give-way violations, and were involved in more give-way accidents. The results draw attention to the problems encountered during car following, and we relate this to the cognitive demands placed on drivers, especially police drivers who often engage in intentional car following and pursuits. 相似文献
959.
960.
This study explored the applicability of previous research (obtained with groups of college-educated women) about the subjective experience of aging in midlife to men and less-educated people. Two-hundred fifty-nine men and women who graduated from a public high school in 1955-1957 retrospectively assessed their feelings of identity certainty, confident power, generativity, and concern about aging for their 60s, 40s, and 20s. Participants reported higher levels of identity certainty, confident power, and concern about aging at each age, and a leveling off of generativity in their 60s. There were some gender and social class differences. Although men and women recalled the same trajectory of these feelings, men reported higher levels of identity certainty and confident power across age. Non-college-educated men recalled the highest levels of concern about aging across age. We discuss how these findings add to our understanding of the experience of aging in these domains. 相似文献