全文获取类型
收费全文 | 67760篇 |
免费 | 1163篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
68931篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 953篇 |
2019年 | 1193篇 |
2018年 | 1379篇 |
2017年 | 1402篇 |
2016年 | 1677篇 |
2015年 | 1209篇 |
2014年 | 1285篇 |
2013年 | 6007篇 |
2012年 | 2192篇 |
2011年 | 2412篇 |
2010年 | 1432篇 |
2009年 | 1439篇 |
2008年 | 2152篇 |
2007年 | 2109篇 |
2006年 | 1885篇 |
2005年 | 1730篇 |
2004年 | 1671篇 |
2003年 | 1618篇 |
2002年 | 1623篇 |
2001年 | 1993篇 |
2000年 | 1877篇 |
1999年 | 1473篇 |
1998年 | 859篇 |
1997年 | 785篇 |
1996年 | 697篇 |
1995年 | 677篇 |
1994年 | 645篇 |
1993年 | 654篇 |
1992年 | 1149篇 |
1991年 | 1092篇 |
1990年 | 1072篇 |
1989年 | 956篇 |
1988年 | 905篇 |
1987年 | 911篇 |
1986年 | 928篇 |
1985年 | 1016篇 |
1984年 | 830篇 |
1983年 | 721篇 |
1982年 | 599篇 |
1981年 | 566篇 |
1980年 | 561篇 |
1979年 | 760篇 |
1978年 | 601篇 |
1976年 | 554篇 |
1975年 | 699篇 |
1974年 | 729篇 |
1973年 | 691篇 |
1972年 | 603篇 |
1968年 | 572篇 |
1967年 | 572篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
841.
842.
843.
David Fryer 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》1994,4(1):11-14
Demystification, eschewing roles that disempower others and enhancing accessibility to the tools of knowledge, are consistent with the aims of community and applied social psychology. Although a focus on psychological empowerment can be a distraction from the need for actual power, it can also enhance it. Psychologists who avoid victim blaming and individual causal attribution biases can contribute to the discourse about social change. This requires both a reinterpretation of the known facts, and a reframing of social analysis. Social scientists and professionals are today's theologians and priests. Our power lies in our legitimation as interpreters of social reality. Teaching the psychology of empowerment, participative decision making and informed consent can influence the powerful as well as the powerless. 相似文献
844.
Jonathan A. Smith 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》1994,4(4):253-260
This paper considers some of the implications of reflexivity for the practice of psychological research. After a brief consideration of the theoretical background to a concern with reflexivity, the paper goes on to examine how various researchers have attempted to incorporate components of reflexivity in their research practice. While a group of sociologists of science have operationalized reflexivity by self-conscious concern for their own role in the construction of social—scientific knowledge, new paradigm co-operative inquirers interpret a need for reflexivity by including their participants as fully self-reflexive co-researchers. Mulkay's suggestion for a dialogic analytical exchange with one's participants is also considered. The second half of the paper illustrates an attempt at reflexive practice in the authors' own research. This was a project concerned with identity change during the transition to motherhood, and aimed to allow the women a strong hand in helping to shape the project's direction. Preliminary analysis of a woman's data was taken back to her for her comments, and her reflections on the data were incorporated in the final case study. A detailed example of the investigator and participant's discussion of a piece of data is provided. 相似文献
845.
This article reports on the factor structure of theScale to Assess World View
(c) (SAWV). Alpha reliabilities for the factors are presented. Gender differences in world views are also explored to understand how
men and women in this sample viewed the world. Recommendations for client assessment, therapy, education and training, and
scale revision are discussed.
The research reported here is based on a paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC, August 17, 1992. 相似文献
846.
Dominick A. Rizzi 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1994,15(3):315-333
Background: Causal reasoning as a way to make a diagnosis seems convincing. Modern medicine depends on the search for causes of disease and it seems fair to assert that such knowledge is employed in diagnosis. Causal reasoning as it has been presented neglects to some extent the conception of multifactorial disease causes. Goal: The purpose of this paper is to analyze aspects of causation relevant for discussing causal reasoning in a diagnostic context. Procedures: The analysis will discuss different conceptions of causal reasoning in medical diagnosis, discriminating primarily between narrow causal diagnosis and more thorough causal explanation. The theory of causes as non-redundant factors in effective causal complexes is used as an analytical background. Causal explanations are performed according to different causal models. Such models of diagnosis are assumptions concerning structure and mechanisms, which cannot be directly or immediately observed. Conceptions and results of causal search strategies differ, according to the focus of the searcher. Causal reasoning is also seen in diagnosis in a more extensive meaning: the pin-pointing of factors responsible for the condition of the patient at any time during the course of disease. Conclusion: Causal reasoning and diagnosis go well in hand, especially if both concepts are widened. The theory of causes as non-redundant components in effective causal complexes, modulated by what is referred to as the stop problem and causal fields, is valuable for explaining the many aspects of causal reasoning in medical diagnosis. 相似文献
847.
Over the past two decades, homographs have been used in psychological experiments aimed at testing a variety of theoretical issues concerning memory and language. Often, such research requires prior knowledge of the dominance relations among various meanings of the homographs. Previously available homograph meaning norms are limited because they are now more than 10 years old, and they have typically reported only the two most dominant meanings even though many homographs have three or more common meanings. This paper presents normative data on 120 homographs from a relatively large, heterogeneous sample of subjects (N = 100). Meaning dominance was assessed by having subjects write the first definition that came to mind for each homograph. Definition responses were grouped by similarity, and the resulting meaning categories were verified against dictionary meaning classifications. The number of distinct meanings varied from two to six for the homographs investigated, and frequency of response is reported for all definition categories. 相似文献
848.
849.
Claude A. M. Valiquette Alain D. Lesage Mireille Cyr Jean Toupin 《Behavior research methods》1994,26(1):60-61
This short paper proposes a general computing strategy to compute Kappa coefficients using the SPSS MATRIX routine. The method is based on the following rationale. If the contingency table is considered as a square matrix, then the observed proportions of agreement lie in the main diagonal’s cells, and their sum equals the trace of the matrix, whereas the proportions of agreement expected by chance are the joint product of marginals. The generalization to weighted kappa, which requires an additional square matrix of disagreement weights, both matrices having the same order, becomes possible by the use of the Hadamard product-that is, the elementwise direct product of two matrices. 相似文献
850.
In three experiments, subjects made shape discriminations of three-dimensional objects differing in orientation, number of bends, and location of bends (e.g., the central arm vs. a minor subarm). In general, encoding times at 0° disparity on bothsame anddifferent trials were affected by the number of bends, but only after a certain threshold of bends in the objects had been reached (Experiment 1). This effect was not due to the subjects’ having to search for matching ends of the objects (Experiment 2). In contrast, rotation rates were influenced by the location of the bends, but not by the number of bends per se (Experiment 3). The results support a representational scheme that is hierarchical, but not necessarily one in which the principal axis of an object is paramount. 相似文献