首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   85338篇
  免费   2819篇
  国内免费   31篇
  2020年   853篇
  2019年   1021篇
  2018年   4447篇
  2017年   3877篇
  2016年   3451篇
  2015年   1251篇
  2014年   1399篇
  2013年   6197篇
  2012年   2908篇
  2011年   4510篇
  2010年   3718篇
  2009年   2859篇
  2008年   3627篇
  2007年   4019篇
  2006年   1978篇
  2005年   1878篇
  2004年   1836篇
  2003年   1743篇
  2002年   1614篇
  2001年   2161篇
  2000年   2048篇
  1999年   1551篇
  1998年   810篇
  1997年   731篇
  1996年   769篇
  1995年   697篇
  1994年   683篇
  1993年   657篇
  1992年   1231篇
  1991年   1144篇
  1990年   1115篇
  1989年   1055篇
  1988年   1012篇
  1987年   961篇
  1986年   971篇
  1985年   1048篇
  1984年   871篇
  1983年   749篇
  1982年   588篇
  1981年   584篇
  1979年   840篇
  1978年   663篇
  1977年   567篇
  1976年   565篇
  1975年   688篇
  1974年   775篇
  1973年   782篇
  1972年   659篇
  1971年   601篇
  1968年   635篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Godden  David 《Topoi》2019,38(4):725-750
Topoi - Robert Fogelin argued that the efficacy of our acts of reasons-giving depends on the normalcy of our discourse—to the extent that discourse is not normal disagreements occurring in it...  相似文献   
62.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
63.
64.
65.
Previous work on children's intuitive knowledge about the natural world has documented their difficulty in acquiring an overarching concept of biological life that includes plants as well as humans and non‐human animals. It has also suggested that the acquisition of fundamental biological concepts like alive and die may be influenced by the language used to describe them, as evidenced by differences between English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children's performance in tasks involving these concepts. Here, we examine one particularly important source of linguistic information available to children during this acquisition process: everyday conversations with their parents. We take a cross‐linguistic approach in analysing the evidence available to English‐ and Indonesian‐speaking children as they acquire meanings for words corresponding to the concepts alive and die . Our analysis illustrates that young children acquiring English and Indonesian are faced with distinct problems, but that parental input in both languages does little to support the acquisition of broad, inclusive biological concepts.  相似文献   
66.
67.
Logical connectives, such as “AND”, “OR”, “IF . . . THEN”, and “IF AND ONLY IF” are ubiquitous in both language and cognition; however, reasoning with logical connectives is error-prone. We argue that some of these errors may stem from people's tendency to minimize the number of possibilities compatible with logical connectives and to construct a “minimalist” one-possibility representation. As a result, connectives denoting a single possibility (e.g., conjunctions) are likely to be represented correctly, whereas connectives denoting multiple possibilities (e.g., disjunctions or conditionals) are likely to be erroneously represented as conjunctions. These predictions were tested and confirmed in three experiments using different paradigms. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with a multiple-choice task and asked to select all and only those possibilities that would indicate that compound verbal propositions were true versus false. In Experiment 2, a somewhat similar task was used, except that participants were asked later to perform a cued recall of verbal propositions. Finally, Experiment 3 used an old/new recognition paradigm to examine participants' ability to accurately recognize different logical connectives. The results of the three experiments are discussed in relation to theories of representation of possibilities and theories of reasoning.  相似文献   
68.
69.
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号