全文获取类型
收费全文 | 33990篇 |
免费 | 222篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
34214篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 76篇 |
2021年 | 123篇 |
2020年 | 221篇 |
2019年 | 272篇 |
2018年 | 3725篇 |
2017年 | 3087篇 |
2016年 | 2549篇 |
2015年 | 453篇 |
2014年 | 422篇 |
2013年 | 1623篇 |
2012年 | 1151篇 |
2011年 | 2958篇 |
2010年 | 2833篇 |
2009年 | 1845篇 |
2008年 | 2201篇 |
2007年 | 2656篇 |
2006年 | 592篇 |
2005年 | 720篇 |
2004年 | 636篇 |
2003年 | 603篇 |
2002年 | 545篇 |
2001年 | 272篇 |
2000年 | 270篇 |
1999年 | 231篇 |
1998年 | 221篇 |
1997年 | 231篇 |
1996年 | 201篇 |
1995年 | 170篇 |
1994年 | 160篇 |
1993年 | 155篇 |
1992年 | 141篇 |
1991年 | 153篇 |
1990年 | 141篇 |
1989年 | 121篇 |
1988年 | 88篇 |
1987年 | 118篇 |
1986年 | 127篇 |
1985年 | 165篇 |
1984年 | 165篇 |
1983年 | 140篇 |
1982年 | 133篇 |
1981年 | 145篇 |
1980年 | 124篇 |
1979年 | 110篇 |
1978年 | 120篇 |
1977年 | 108篇 |
1976年 | 108篇 |
1975年 | 97篇 |
1974年 | 105篇 |
1973年 | 88篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Pezdek, Blandon-Gitlin, and Gabbay (2006) found that perceptions of the plausibility of events increase the likelihood that
imagination may induce false memories of those events. Using a survey conducted by Gallup, we asked a large sample of the
general population how plausible it would be for a person with longstanding emotional problems and a need for psychotherapy
to be a victim of childhood sexual abuse, even though the person could not remember the abuse. Only 18% indicated that it
was implausible or very implausible, whereas 67% indicated that such an occurrence was either plausible or very plausible.
Combined with Pezdek et al.’s findings, and counter to their conclusions, our findings imply that there is a substantial danger
of inducing false memories of childhood sexual abuse through imagination in psychotherapy. 相似文献
952.
Clifasefi SL Garry M Harper DN Sharman SJ Sutherland R 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2007,14(1):112-117
Can a placebo for a psychotropic drug help participants resist the misinformation effect? To answer this question, we gave participants a mixture of baking soda and water and told half of them that the mixture was a cognition-enhancing drug called R273 and told the other half that it was an inactive version of the drug. Shortly thereafter, all participants took part in a three-stage misinformation experiment. Compared with participants who were told that they had taken the placebo, the participants who were told that they had taken the drug reported improved cognitive abilities and were less susceptible to the misinformation effect. We provide sourcemonitoring and mindfulness accounts of our findings. 相似文献
953.
When sequences of discrete events, or other units, are independently coded by two coders using a set of mutually exclusive
and exhaustive codes, but the onset times for the codes are not preserved, it is often unclear how pairs of protocols should
be aligned. Yet such alignment is required before Cohen’s kappa, a common agreement statistic, can be computed. Here we describe
a method—based on the Needleman and Wunsch (1970) algorithm originally devised for aligning nucleotide sequences—for optimally
aligning such sequences; we also offer the results of a simulation study of the behavior of alignment kappa with a number
of variables, including number of codes, varying degrees of observer accuracy, sequence length, code variability, and parameters
governing the alignment algorithm. We conclude that (1) under most reasonable circumstances, observer accuracies of 90% or
better result in alignment kappas of .60 or better; (2) generally, alignment kappas are not strongly affected by sequence
length, the number of codes, or the variability in the codes’ probability; (3) alignment kappas are adversely affected when
missed events and false alarms are possible; and (4) cost matrices and priority orders used in the algorithm should favor
substitutions (i.e., disagreements) over insertions and deletions (i.e., missed events and false alarms). Two computer programs
were developed: Global Sequence Alignment, or GSA, for carrying out the simulation study, and Event Alignment, or ELign, a
user-oriented program that computes alignment kappa and provides the optimal alignment given a pair of event sequences. 相似文献
954.
One major advantage of Web-based research lies in its ability to reach and study people who have rare conditions of interest.
Another advantage is that, due to the anonymity of the survey situation, the Internet is particularly suited for surveys on
sensitive topics. Sexsomnia is a newly identified medical condition whose sufferers engage in sexual behavior during their
sleep. Problematic cases are highly distressing and have forensic implications. The consensus among opinion leaders in sleep
medicine is that sexsomnia may be quite common but that it often goes unreported because of shame and embarrassment. Thus,
little is known about this condition’s demographics and clinical features. This article reports findings from a sample analysis
of 20 years of research on sexsomnia and discusses the results, strengths, and weaknesses of a recent Web-based survey conducted
on the difficult-to-reach clinical population that suffers from sexsomnia. 相似文献
955.
Using a speeded retrieval procedure, we investigated time-of-day effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Morning-type
adults were tested at either peak (early morning) or off-peak (late afternoon) times on a speeded implicit (Experiment 1)
or explicit (Experiment 2) stem completion task. In Experiment 1, retrieval strategies were identified by changes in response
speed between a practice phase with rapid retrieval and an implicit memory test phase. Performance based on controlled retrieval
(shown by slowdown participants) showed more priming at peak than at off-peak times of day, a finding confirmed in Experiment
2, in which the participants were given intentional retrieval instructions when the materials switched. In contrast, performance
based on automatic retrieval (shown by nonslowdown participants) did not differ across peak and off-peak times. The finding
suggests a robust synchrony effect in controlled retrieval, but not in automatic retrieval, which does not appear to vary
across the day. 相似文献
956.
Two experiments examine how collaboration influences visual search performance. Working with a partner or on their own, participants
reported whether a target was present or absent in briefly presented search displays. We compared the search performance of
individuals working together (collaborative pairs) with the pooled responses of the individuals working alone (nominal pairs).
Collaborative pairs were less likely than nominal pairs to correctly detect a target and they were less likely to make false
alarms. Signal detection analyses revealed that collaborative pairs were more sensitive to the presence of the target and
had a more conservative response bias than the nominal pairs. This pattern was observed even when the presence of another
individual was matched across pairs. The results are discussed in the context of task-sharing, social loafing and current
theories of visual search. 相似文献
957.
958.
Grant and Spivey (2003) proposed that eye movement trajectories can influence spatial reasoning by way of an implicit eye-movement-to-cognition
link. We tested this proposal and investigated the nature of this link by continuously monitoring eye movements and asking
participants to perform a problem-solving task under free-viewing conditions while occasionally guiding their eye movements
(via an unrelated tracking task), either in a pattern related to the problem’s solution or in unrelated patterns. Although
participants reported that they were not aware of any relationship between the tracking task and the problem, those who moved
their eyes in a pattern related to the problem’s solution were the most successful problem solvers. Our results support the
existence of an implicit compatibility between spatial cognition and the eye movement patterns that people use to examine
a scene. 相似文献
959.
Children sometimes have trouble switching from one task to another, despite demonstrating an awareness of current task demands.
This behavior could reflect problems either directly inhibiting previously relevant information or sufficiently activating
graded working memory representations for the current task. We tested competing predictions from each account, using a computerized
card-sorting task in which we assessed children’s task switching abilities and their response speed to simple questions about
current task demands. All children answered these questions correctly, but children who successfully switched tasks responded
more quickly to questions than did children who perseverated on previous tasks, even after factoring out processing speed
and age. This reaction time difference supports graded working memory accounts, with stronger representations of current task
demands aiding both task-switching and responses to questions. This result poses a challenge for directed inhibition accounts,
because nothing needs to be inhibited to answer simple questions that lack conflicting information. 相似文献
960.
Joaquin Trujillo 《Human Studies》2007,30(4):345-356
This is an existential-phenomenological reading of Max Weber’s “Class, Status, Party” that seeks a fuller understanding of meaning accomplishment in a stratified World. I appropriate stratification as a single meaning structure ontically defined by domination, intersubjectivity, and life-chances and ontologically determined by the power-to-be (Seinkönnen), There-being-with-others (Mitdasein), and potentiality (Möglichkeit). I then discuss the significance of these structures in finite transcendence (There-being, Dasein) and describe ways they factually unfold in World achievement. I conclude with logotherapeutic reflections concerning meaning accomplishment in a stratified World and a summary of key questions facing existential-phenomenology in light of the likelihood that There-being must embrace, indeed, live, the inherent equality of Being (Gleichheit des Seins) among Daseins to accomplish its authenticity. 相似文献