全文获取类型
收费全文 | 314篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
332篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 30篇 |
2012年 | 17篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有332条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Anna Saltini Daniela Rebecchi Chiara Callerame Isabel Fernandez Elisa Bergonzini Fabrizio Starace 《Psychology, health & medicine》2018,23(3):285-294
‘Early psychological intervention’ is defined as commencing treatment within three months of the traumatic event, with the aim to prevent or treat posttraumatic stress disorder, ongoing distress or acute stress disorder. In natural disaster situations, specific issues may limit the amount of time available for treatment and the possibility of interventions. Eye Movement Desensitisation and Reprocessing (EMDR) can be used without regard to these limits. The aim of the study is to evaluate the effects of EMDR, Recent Traumatic Episode Protocol (R-TEP) provided within three months of the traumatic event to a large sample of individuals exposed to the earthquake that hit Emilia Romagna Region (Northern Italy) in 2012. This study is based on a retrospective review of medical records collected during the activities of psychological and psychosocial unit in the immediate aftermath of earthquake. In total, 529 participants completed the Impact of Event Scale Revised (IES-R) (pre e post treatment). In order to provide a comparison similar to a waitlist-like control group, a method of cohort analysis was applied. In addition, possible time dependent effect was tested. ET (early-treated sample, participants treated within one month after the earthquake) and LT (late-treated sample, participants treated after the first month from the earthquake) reported at post-treatment an improvement to a level below the IES-R cutoff (65.8% of the ET sample and 64.02% of the LT sample). Control group analogue and time-outcome correlation suggest that positive changes in symptoms were likely due to the treatment provided and not merely to the time lapse from the traumatic event. The results of this study suggest that EMDR is a viable treatment option in response to a disaster crisis and in reducing psychological distress of acutely traumatized individuals within the context of a natural disaster. 相似文献
92.
Verdonschot RG La Heij W Paolieri D Zhang Q Schiller NO 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(9):1836-1849
The current study investigated the effects of phonologically related context pictures on the naming latencies of target words in Japanese and Chinese. Reading bare words in alphabetic languages has been shown to be rather immune to effects of context stimuli, even when these stimuli are presented in advance of the target word (e.g., Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984 ; Roelofs, 2003 ). However, recently, semantic context effects of distractor pictures on the naming latencies of Japanese kanji (but not Chinese hànzì) words have been observed (Verdonschot, La Heij, & Schiller, 2010 ). In the present study, we further investigated this issue using phonologically related (i.e., homophonic) context pictures when naming target words in either Chinese or Japanese. We found that pronouncing bare nouns in Japanese is sensitive to phonologically related context pictures, whereas this is not the case in Chinese. The difference between these two languages is attributed to processing costs caused by multiple pronunciations for Japanese kanji. 相似文献
93.
Daniela Marrani 《Nanoethics》2013,7(3):177-188
Food is a big business in the EU and nanofood products are beginning to be placed on the market. It is still unclear whether the absence of minimum regulation at a global level promotes or prevents the growth of a market in nanofood. However, the development of an adequate risk management policy in relation to food safety is a key concern for consumers. Importantly, the European Parliament in its 2009 Resolution on “Legal aspects on nanomaterials” called for more in-depth scientific research on the toxicity of compounds in nanomaterials, and for the adoption of an EU definition of nanomaterials for regulatory purposes. Unfortunately, in 2011, nanotechnology led to inconclusive debates in the context of the revision of Novel Food Regulation. General Food Law applies to nanofood in terms of safety requirements, and specific rules also apply to food contact materials containing nanoparticles as well as to to additives, vitamins and minerals. The EU legislator also introduced mandatory labelling in respect of products derived from nanotechnologies. The legal framework is evolving according to the so-called “incremental approach”, a governance model that creates the risk of fragmentation. But the main problem is the inconsistent definition of the terms “nanotechnology” and “nanomaterials” when looking at the enforcement of regulations and the provision of a wide range of specific tools for different nanofoods: for example the use of positive lists of authorised substances applying only to food contact materials, additives and supplements. This contribution aims to review the regulations in force in respect to nanofood and novel foods and to highlight the problems that are still unresolved. 相似文献
94.
95.
Ana‐Maria Bliuc Craig McGarty Katherine Reynolds Daniela Muntele 《European journal of social psychology》2007,37(1):19-32
Research on group identification has shown it to be a surprisingly weak predictor of intentions to take large‐scale social action. The weak links may exist because researchers have not always examined identification with the type of group that is most relevant for predicting action. Our focus in two studies (one in Romania and one in Australia, both Ns = 101) was on opinion‐based groups (i.e. groups formed around shared opinions). We found that social identification with opinion‐based groups was an excellent predictor of political behavioural intentions, particularly when items measuring identity certainty were included. The results provide clear evidence of the role of social identity constructs for predicting commitment to social action and complement analyses of politicised collective identity and crowd behaviour. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
96.
97.
Ellen M. Migo Adrian Roper Daniela Montaldi Andrew R. Mayes 《Behavior research methods》2010,42(2):470-473
Word stem completion tasks involve showing participants a number of words and then later asking them to complete word stems
to make a full word. If the stem is completed with one of the studied words, it indicates memory. It is a test widely used
to assess both implicit and explicit forms of memory. An important aspect of stimulus selection is that target words should
not frequently be generated spontaneously from the word stem, to ensure that production of the word really represents memory.
In this article, we present a database of spontaneous stem completion rates for 395 stems from a group of 80 British undergraduate
psychology students. It includes information on other characteristics of the words (word frequency, concreteness, imageability,
age of acquisition, common part of speech, and number of letters) and, as such, can be used to select suitable words to include
in a stem completion task. Supplemental materials for this article may be downloaded from http://brm .psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
98.
99.
100.
Daniela Raccanello Margherita Brondino Margherita Pasini 《Journal of Beliefs & Values》2015,36(2):231-238
Pride and shame, two moral emotions partially neglected by the literature, can also be categorised as achievement emotions, because of their salience within everyday school and academic life. Our aims were to explore whether they are organised in a setting-specific manner and whether they differ in two settings, learning exam-relevant material and taking exams. Five hundred and forty university students evaluated pride and shame related to the two settings. A confirmatory multitrait-multimethod analysis (MTMM) indicated higher salience of type of emotion rather than type of setting; however, the structure of pride and shame was best explained when also type of setting was accounted for. A repeated-measure analysis of variance revealed higher levels of the positive emotion compared to the negative emotion, with the latter higher for the evaluative setting. Findings are discussed in light of their theoretical and applied relevance. 相似文献