全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5784篇 |
免费 | 298篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
6084篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 58篇 |
2022年 | 49篇 |
2021年 | 72篇 |
2020年 | 129篇 |
2019年 | 179篇 |
2018年 | 214篇 |
2017年 | 230篇 |
2016年 | 216篇 |
2015年 | 180篇 |
2014年 | 189篇 |
2013年 | 627篇 |
2012年 | 315篇 |
2011年 | 340篇 |
2010年 | 208篇 |
2009年 | 207篇 |
2008年 | 284篇 |
2007年 | 265篇 |
2006年 | 224篇 |
2005年 | 197篇 |
2004年 | 184篇 |
2003年 | 181篇 |
2002年 | 161篇 |
2001年 | 77篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 57篇 |
1998年 | 76篇 |
1997年 | 65篇 |
1996年 | 65篇 |
1995年 | 47篇 |
1994年 | 62篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 48篇 |
1991年 | 44篇 |
1990年 | 48篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1988年 | 34篇 |
1987年 | 34篇 |
1986年 | 30篇 |
1985年 | 44篇 |
1984年 | 39篇 |
1982年 | 45篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 41篇 |
1979年 | 35篇 |
1978年 | 37篇 |
1977年 | 42篇 |
1976年 | 35篇 |
1975年 | 33篇 |
1974年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 26篇 |
排序方式: 共有6084条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Human intuition is a rich and useful guide to uncertain events in the environment but suffers from probabilistic incoherence in the technical sense. Developing methods for extracting a coherent body of judgement that is maximally consistent with a person's intuition is a challenging task for cognitive psychology, and also relevant to the construction of artificial expert systems. The present article motivates this problem, and outlines one approach to it. 相似文献
52.
53.
Chronic Thought Suppression 总被引:18,自引:0,他引:18
ABSTRACT We conducted several tests of the idea that an inclination toward thought suppression is associated with obsessive thinking and emotional reactivity. Initially, we developed a self-report measure of thought suppression through successive factor-analytic procedures and found that it exhibited acceptable internal consistency and temporal stability. This measure, the White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI), was found to correlate with measures of obsessional thinking and depressive and anxious affect, to predict signs of clinical obsession among individuals prone toward obsessional thinking, to predict depression among individuals motivated to dislike negative thoughts, and to predict failure of electrodermal responses to habituate among people having emotional thoughts. The WBSI was inversely correlated with repression as assessed by the Repression-Sensitization Scale, and so taps a trait that is quite unlike repression as traditionally conceived. 相似文献
54.
Origins of Ruminative Thought: Trauma, Incompleteness, Nondisclosure, and Suppression 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
The purpose of this paper is to discuss theories of the origin of ruminative thought. We begin by providing a working definition of rumination, separating rumination from other forms of cognitive activity and distinguishing ruminations from ordinary memories. Then, we review what we believe are the major categories of theory that attempt to account for the existence and nature of rumination. These include theories of traumatization, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and thought suppression. Ruminations may originate for a number of reasons, but it seems they may continue because of our attempts to control them. Evidence from studies on thought suppression suggests that the suppression of unwanted thoughts may in fact fuel the very emotions and thoughts we are trying to avoid. Thought suppression may set up a state in which we not only increase the amount we think about an unwanted thought, but potentially also sharpen our emotional reaction to those thoughts. 相似文献
55.
56.
Daniel Alroy 《Synthese》1995,104(1):147-160
Neural impulses from the senses to the brain convey information, not sensation. The direct electrical stimulation of the cortex produces sensations. Hence, such sensations are evoked in the brain, and not received from the senses, nor from the outside world through the senses. More specifically, the experience of light is evoked in the brain and not received from the eyes. Consequently, the born blind, too, would experience light in response to electrical brain stimulation. The luminosity of light is not a property of electromagnetic radiation. If the experience of light is private, then so are the visual observations it makes possible. 相似文献
57.
This study compares and contrasts the use of gender-related concepts in other-and self-perception. The schematic use and stereotypic and affective meanings of feminine and masculine were derived from hierarchical classes analysis (HICLAS) of participants' (N = 72; 50 females, 22 males, 54% white) ratings of self and significant others. The study reveals that people use these concepts differently in their perception of others than in their perception of self. In addition, the meaning of masculine and feminine constructed by these participants differs significantly from their traditional definitions. Finally, being feminine is evaluated more positively than being masculine in the perception of others, while the ego protective function of gender leads participants to evaluate their own gender's features most positively in self-perception.This article was presented at the 1993 Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, DC, April 1993. The collection of the data reported in this paper was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH40871-03/04. We thank the Institute for its support. We also thank the participants in this study for sharing their lives with us. Finally we would like to thank Richard Ashmore, Rae Carlson, and Basil Englis for comments on earlier drafts of this paper. 相似文献
58.
IMPLICIT MEMORY IN AMNESIC PATIENTS: 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Abstract— Amnesic patients generally exhibit spared priming effects on implicit memory tasks despite poor explicit memory In a previous study, we demonstrated normal auditory priming in amnesic patients on an identification-in-noise test in which the magnitude of priming is independent of whether the speaker's voice is the same or different at study and test In the present experiment, we examined auditory priming on a filter identification test in which the magnitude of priming in control subjects is higher when the speaker's voice is the same at study and test than when it is different Amnesic patients, by contrast, failed to exhibit more priming in a same-voice condition than in a different-voice condition Voice-specific priming may depend on a memory system that is impaired in amnesia 相似文献
59.
The impact of informational feedback on the depositing of aluminum cans in recycling receptacles at a medium-sized university was investigated. Informational signs were placed over 20-recycling receptacles in three academic buildings. Each informational sign was placed at eye level, with the number of aluminum cans deposited for recycling during the previous week displayed in red digits. A 65% increase in the number of aluminum cans deposited in recycling receptacles with the informational signs was observed. Furthermore, removal of the informational signs was observed to reliably decrease the number of aluminum cans deposited. These results are discussed in the context of employing feedback to increase environmental protection behaviors. 相似文献
60.
Three experiments examined repetition priming for meaningful environmental sounds (e.g., clock ticking, tooth brushing, toilet flushing, etc.) in a sound stem identification paradigm using brief sound cues. Prior encoding of target sounds together with their associated names facilitated subsequent identification of sound stems relative to nonstudied controls. In contrast, prior exposure to names alone in the absence of the environmental sounds did not prime subsequent sound stem identification performance at all (Experiments 1 and 3). Explicit and implicit memory were dissociated such that sound stem cued recall was higher following semantic than nonsemantic encoding, whereas sound priming was insensitive to manipulations of depth encoding (Experiments 2 and 3). These results extend the findings of long-term repetition priming into the auditory nonverbal domain and suggest that priming for environmental sounds is mediated primarily by perceptual processes. 相似文献