全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5527篇 |
免费 | 292篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
5821篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 57篇 |
2022年 | 47篇 |
2021年 | 69篇 |
2020年 | 122篇 |
2019年 | 172篇 |
2018年 | 206篇 |
2017年 | 220篇 |
2016年 | 204篇 |
2015年 | 177篇 |
2014年 | 182篇 |
2013年 | 583篇 |
2012年 | 300篇 |
2011年 | 328篇 |
2010年 | 204篇 |
2009年 | 208篇 |
2008年 | 266篇 |
2007年 | 259篇 |
2006年 | 215篇 |
2005年 | 188篇 |
2004年 | 174篇 |
2003年 | 178篇 |
2002年 | 152篇 |
2001年 | 77篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 54篇 |
1998年 | 71篇 |
1997年 | 61篇 |
1996年 | 63篇 |
1995年 | 45篇 |
1994年 | 58篇 |
1993年 | 42篇 |
1992年 | 42篇 |
1991年 | 41篇 |
1990年 | 45篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 43篇 |
1984年 | 38篇 |
1982年 | 46篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 39篇 |
1979年 | 35篇 |
1978年 | 36篇 |
1977年 | 40篇 |
1976年 | 35篇 |
1975年 | 31篇 |
1974年 | 22篇 |
1973年 | 27篇 |
排序方式: 共有5821条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Harry R.M. Purser Emily K. Farran Yannick Courbois Axelle Lemahieu Pascal Sockeel Daniel Mellier Mark Blades 《Developmental science》2015,18(4):599-613
The ability to navigate new environments has a significant impact on the daily life and independence of people with learning difficulties. The aims of this study were to investigate the development of route learning in Down syndrome (N = 50), Williams syndrome (N = 19), and typically developing children between 5 and 11 years old (N = 108); to investigate use of landmarks; and to relate cognitive functions to route‐learning ability in these groups. Overall, measures of attention and long‐term memory were strongly associated with route learning, even once non‐verbal ability was controlled for. All of the groups, including 5‐ to 6‐year‐old TD children, demonstrated the ability to make use of all landmark types to aid route learning; those near junctions, those further from junctions, and also distant landmarks (e.g. church spire, radio mast). Individuals with WS performed better than a matched subset of TD children on more difficult routes; we suggest that this is supported by relatively strong visual feature recognition in the disorder. Participants with DS who had relatively high levels of non‐verbal ability performed at a similar level to TD participants. 相似文献
212.
The terms du (獨) and shendu (慎獨) frequently appear in transmitted texts, notably, among others, the Xunzi and Liji. Drawing reference from the poetry of “Shijiu” (鳲鳩) (Ode 152) and “Yanyan” (燕燕) (Ode 28) in the Book of Odes (詩經), the recovered texts of “Wuxing Commentary” (五行 說) and “Confucian Poetics” (孔子詩論) have provided new material for re-shaping our current understanding of the concepts of du and shendu. This study will briefly survey the semantic ranges of these terms within the exegetical tradition and explore their meaning with regard to the poetry from which they are contextualized. In the final analysis du can be understood as the ontic quality of the heart-mind within the broad sense of cheng (誠 sincerity), or devout love, whereas shendu can be regarded as a process of moral cultivation. To some extent the re-interpretation of these terms finds commonality with, rather than subverts, the semantic ranges established by traditional glosses. The recovered texts have enhanced our understanding of these terms, in particular the concepts of heart-mind and emotion in early China. 相似文献
213.
214.
Bill Himadi Frances Osteen Andrew J. Kaiser Kimberly Daniel 《Behavioral Interventions》1991,6(5):355-366
This study examined the strength of delusional beliefs during the modification of delusional verbalizations of a 51 year-old chronically mentally ill inpatient. The changing-criterion design was used to document the effectiveness of a behavioral treatment package to alter the rate of delusional responses to personal background questions. The results revealed a stepwise decline in the frequency of delusional responses. Generalization effects to novel interviewers were obtained. No changes were obtained on the subject's ratings of delusional beliefs, with the subject assigning ratings of absolute conviction to his beliefs throughout the duration of the study. 相似文献
215.
A. Daniel Yarmey 《Journal of applied social psychology》1991,21(22):1868-1876
Following a telephone survey investigating attitudes toward police and frequency of personal victimization. 177 citizens between the ages of 18 and 88 years participated without forewarning in a field study on telephone voice identification. Identification accuracy (hits) improved with longer conversation durations (an average duration of 7.8 min compared to conversation durations of 3.2 min and 4.3 min. on average), but the false alarm rate also reliably increased with longer durations in a target-present lineup. False alarms were consistently high (overall M= .48) in the target-absent lineup. No significant differences were found in hit scores or false alarm scores over a two to three day retention interval. No significant correlation was found between confidence scores and accuracy of performance on the target-present lineup (r= .10). but a significant correlation (r= 36) was found on the target-absent lineup. 相似文献
216.
Wilma Koutstaal Daniel L. Schacter Marcia K. Johnson Lissa Galluccio 《Memory & cognition》1999,27(3):478-493
Selectively reviewing some items from a larger set of previously learned items increases memory for the items that are reviewed but may also be accompanied by a cost: Memory for the nonreviewed items may be impaired relative to cases where no review occurs at all. This cost to nonreviewed items has primarily been shown in contexts of verbal list learning and in situations where the reviewed and nonreviewed items are categorically or semantically related. Using a more naturalistic impetus to selective review--photographs relating to previously experienced events--we assessed whether the memory of older and younger adults for unrelated complex activities that they themselves had performed was also impaired due to nonreview. Both younger and older adults showed impaired memory for nonreviewed activities when tested with free recall (Experiment 1), but not when tested with recognition or cued recall (Experiment 2). If mitigating retrieval cues are unavailable, selective review may impair memory for nonreviewed everyday events. 相似文献
217.
Victor W. Henderson Wendy Mack David M. Freed Daniel Kempler Elaine S. Andersen 《Brain and language》1990,39(4):530-538
Although lexical semantic deficits are postulated to play a prominent role in the anomia of Alzheimer's disease, it is unclear whether the primary disturbance is one of lexical access or one of lexical semantic loss. Response consistency on a naming task is one means of evaluating the underlying source of naming impairment. Access dysfunction usually implies variable word-finding difficulty, while a theory of lexical loss predicts that many word names would be consistently unavailable. Nineteen Alzheimer's disease patients were administered a visual confrontation naming task (the Boston Naming Test) on two occasions 6 months apart. Eighty percent of errors occurred consistently at both times; only 20% of errors occurred on only one occasion. Response consistency occurred significantly more often than expected under the assumption of no response consistency. Findings support the hypothesis that anomia in Alzheimer's disease is in part due to a loss of lexical semantic information. 相似文献
218.
219.
The presence of nonobservational vocabulary is shown to be necessary for wide application of a conservative principle of theory revision.Support for this research was provided by the Office of Naval Research under contract No. N00014-87-K-0401. We thank Daniel Andler and Clark Glymour for helpful discussion. 相似文献
220.
The Influence of Verbal Responses to Common Greetings on Compliance Behavior: The Foot-In-The-Mouth Effect 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This study reports the results of three field experiments which demonstrate that asking someone how they feel, having them verbally respond, and then acknowledging that response, facilitates compliance with a charitable request. The findings are discussed with respect to the influence of public commitments on behavioral consistency.
Before you ask anyone for a donation, you first ask them how they're feeling. After they tell you they're feeling good, and you tell them you're glad they're feeling good, the/11 be more likely to contribute to helping someone who isn't. 相似文献
Before you ask anyone for a donation, you first ask them how they're feeling. After they tell you they're feeling good, and you tell them you're glad they're feeling good, the/11 be more likely to contribute to helping someone who isn't. 相似文献