全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5855篇 |
免费 | 111篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
5967篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 41篇 |
2023年 | 58篇 |
2022年 | 50篇 |
2021年 | 75篇 |
2020年 | 126篇 |
2019年 | 180篇 |
2018年 | 207篇 |
2017年 | 222篇 |
2016年 | 213篇 |
2015年 | 181篇 |
2014年 | 187篇 |
2013年 | 593篇 |
2012年 | 304篇 |
2011年 | 332篇 |
2010年 | 211篇 |
2009年 | 206篇 |
2008年 | 269篇 |
2007年 | 261篇 |
2006年 | 217篇 |
2005年 | 187篇 |
2004年 | 177篇 |
2003年 | 180篇 |
2002年 | 156篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 57篇 |
1999年 | 55篇 |
1998年 | 71篇 |
1997年 | 62篇 |
1996年 | 63篇 |
1995年 | 47篇 |
1994年 | 62篇 |
1993年 | 45篇 |
1992年 | 44篇 |
1991年 | 43篇 |
1990年 | 46篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 30篇 |
1986年 | 26篇 |
1985年 | 46篇 |
1984年 | 40篇 |
1982年 | 45篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 39篇 |
1979年 | 36篇 |
1978年 | 37篇 |
1977年 | 40篇 |
1976年 | 35篇 |
1975年 | 33篇 |
1973年 | 29篇 |
排序方式: 共有5967条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Dominguez FJ Lawrence C Halpern EF Drohan B Grinstein G Black DM Smith BL Gadd MA Specht M Kopans DB Moore RH Hughes SS Roche CA Hughes KS 《Journal of genetic counseling》2007,16(3):341-345
The self-reporting of cancer history is becoming increasingly important, as it frequently guides medical decision-making.
We studied the accuracy of personal cancer history using a self-administered questionnaire, comparing the results with the
Tumor Registry at our institution. Among 39,662 records, we identified 3614 women with a single cancer in the Tumor Registry
who reported none or one cancer on their questionnaire. The sensitivity in self-reporting cancers was 85.7%, ranging from
92.1% for breast cancer to 42.9% for leukemia. The accuracy for breast cancer and Hodgkin's Lymphoma was significantly better
than other cancers (p=0.00027, CI: 1.4–3.88). Analysis of patient's characteristics showed that Caucasians reported breast cancer more accurately
than Asian/Pacific Islanders (p=0.008), and those with Jewish ancestry more accurately than non-Jewish (p=0.0435). These results will help us to improve data collection and thus improve medical decision-making. 相似文献
212.
Daniel A. Weiskopf 《Synthese》2007,156(1):161-204
There are good reasons to think natural languages are compositional. But compound nominals (CNs) are largely productive constructions
that have proven highly recalcitrant to compositional semantic analysis. I evaluate existing proposals to treat CNs compositionally
and argue that they are unsuccessful. I then articulate an alternative proposal according to which CNs contain covert indexicals.
Features of the context allow a variety of relations to be expressed using CNs, but this variety is not expressed in the lexicon
or the semantic rules of the language. This proposal accounts for the diversity of contents CNs can be used to express while
preserving compositionality. Finally, I defend this proposal against some recent anti-contextualist arguments. 相似文献
213.
214.
The authors examined quantity-based judgments for up to 10 items for simultaneous and sequential whole sets as well as for sequentially dropped items in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). In Experiment 1, subjects had to choose the larger of 2 quantities presented in 2 separate dishes either simultaneously or 1 dish after the other. Representatives of all species were capable of selecting the larger of 2 quantities in both conditions, even when the quantities were large and the numerical distance between them was small. In Experiment 2, subjects had to select between the same food quantities sequentially dropped into 2 opaque cups so that none of the quantities were ever viewed as a whole. The authors found some evidence (albeit weaker) that subjects were able to select the larger quantity of items. Furthermore, the authors found no performance breakdown with the inclusion of certain quantities. Instead, the ratio between quantities was the best performance predictor. The authors conclude that quantity-based judgments rely on an analogical system, not a discrete object file model or perceptual estimation mechanism, such as subitizing. 相似文献
215.
In a prescient paper Karl Lashley (1951) rejected reflex chaining accounts of the sequencing of behavior and argued instead for a more cognitive account in which behavioral sequences are typically controlled with central plans. An important feature of such plans, according to Lashley, is that they are hierarchical. Lashley offered several sources of evidence for the hierarchical organization for behavioral plans, and others afterward provided more evidence for this hypothesis. We briefly review that evidence here and then shift from a focus on the structure of plans (Lashley's point of concentration) to the processes by which plans are formed in real time. Two principles emerge from the studies we review. One is that plans are not formed from scratch for each successive movement sequence but instead are formed by making whatever changes are needed to distinguish the movement sequence to be performed next from the movement sequence that has just been performed. This plan-modification view is supported by two phenomena discovered in our laboratory: the parameter remapping effect, and the handpath priming effect. The other principle we review is that even single movements appear to be controlled with hierarchically organized plans. At the top level are the starting and goal postures. At the lower level are the intermediate states comprising the transition from the starting posture to the goal posture. The latter principle is supported by another phenomenon discovered in our lab, the end-state comfort effect, and by a computational model of motor planning which accounts for a large number of motor phenomena. Interestingly, the computational model hearkens back to a classical method of generating cartoon animations that relies on the production of keyframes first and the production of interframes (intermediate frames) second. 相似文献
216.
Two experiments examine how collaboration influences visual search performance. Working with a partner or on their own, participants
reported whether a target was present or absent in briefly presented search displays. We compared the search performance of
individuals working together (collaborative pairs) with the pooled responses of the individuals working alone (nominal pairs).
Collaborative pairs were less likely than nominal pairs to correctly detect a target and they were less likely to make false
alarms. Signal detection analyses revealed that collaborative pairs were more sensitive to the presence of the target and
had a more conservative response bias than the nominal pairs. This pattern was observed even when the presence of another
individual was matched across pairs. The results are discussed in the context of task-sharing, social loafing and current
theories of visual search. 相似文献
217.
Daniel J. Navarro 《Journal of mathematical psychology》2007,51(2):85-98
An analysis of the “response scaling” parameter in the Generalized Context Model is presented. In light of the existing debate over the behavior of the model when this parameter is included, three different interpretations are discussed, in order to illustrate the effect of the parameter at the decision level, the category similarity level, and the representational structure level. 相似文献
218.
Forget-Dubois N Boivin M Dionne G Pierce T Tremblay RE Pérusse D 《Infant behavior & development》2007,30(3):453-465
Children's negative characteristics are thought to be a factor in evoking hostile parenting responses. This can result in genotype/environment correlations (rGE) in which children's heritable traits influence the parenting they experience. We did genetic analyses on 292 mothers' self-reported hostile-reactive behaviors toward each of their twins at 5, 18 and 30 months. Finding heritability for a parenting behavior analyzed as a child phenotype is evidence of rGE correlation. The heritability of maternal behavior was modest (29% at 5 months, 0% at 18 months, and 25% at 30 months) and longitudinal analyses indicated that genetic factors at 5 and 30 months were uncorrelated. Common environment factors, probably reflecting characteristics of the mothers, were the main source of variance at the three ages and were highly correlated through time. We concluded that children's heritable characteristics evoked maternal negative response at specific times, but were not responsible for the stability of maternal hostility from infancy to toddlerhood. 相似文献
219.
Daniel Steiner 《The Journal of medical humanities》1980,2(4):203-211
This memorandum was prepared by Daniel Steiner, General Counsel to Harvard University on behalf of the President's Office and distributed to the faculty in October, 1980. It reviews recent Harvard policy with regard to patents and technology transfer. Spurred by recombinant DNA research, at Harvard and elsewhere, benefits and pitfalls of the University's participation as a minor shareholder in a company engaged in research and development are identified. The author notes that “The memorandum has benefited from numerous discussions with members of the faculty and administration and in particular from the comments of President (Derek) Bok and Dean (Henry) Rosovsky.” 相似文献
220.