首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   58232篇
  免费   1533篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2020年   459篇
  2019年   571篇
  2018年   4035篇
  2017年   3447篇
  2016年   2941篇
  2015年   774篇
  2014年   784篇
  2013年   3152篇
  2012年   1826篇
  2011年   3578篇
  2010年   3203篇
  2009年   2215篇
  2008年   2806篇
  2007年   3255篇
  2006年   1098篇
  2005年   1143篇
  2004年   1051篇
  2003年   930篇
  2002年   914篇
  2001年   1215篇
  2000年   1201篇
  1999年   921篇
  1998年   417篇
  1997年   406篇
  1996年   371篇
  1995年   360篇
  1993年   359篇
  1992年   764篇
  1991年   666篇
  1990年   731篇
  1989年   622篇
  1988年   661篇
  1987年   616篇
  1986年   612篇
  1985年   559篇
  1984年   513篇
  1983年   495篇
  1979年   591篇
  1978年   404篇
  1977年   353篇
  1975年   462篇
  1974年   510篇
  1973年   537篇
  1972年   409篇
  1971年   404篇
  1970年   360篇
  1969年   417篇
  1968年   491篇
  1967年   442篇
  1966年   366篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
861.
Lesions of the hypothalamic ventromedial nucleus (VMN) led to an immediate increase in food intake but not to any increase in hoarding activity. Later, when subjects were very obese, they failed to hoard even in response to a 16-hr. deprivation schedule, although this schedule did produce hoarding if body weights were held at, or were brought down to, preoperative levels. These results indicate that the lateral hypothalamic mechanism responsible for hoarding and feeding responds to long-term nutritional factors, and that it is not directly affected by the short-term satiety mechanisms in the VMN. These findings support the hypothesis (a) that even “non-physiological” activities (e.g. hoarding, exploration) are motivated by physiological needs, and (b) that the reason these activities do not ordinarily covary with physiological drives is that, unlike the physiological drives, they are not subject to inhibition by the hypothalamic satiety mechanisms.  相似文献   
862.
863.
Freedom from restrictive assumptions and ease of psychological interpretation are two basic criteria for a satisfactory measure of recognition performance. The A-index (Brown, 1965a) comes close to fulfilling these criteria. It is based on the proportion of wrong choices rejected in a multi-choice test as revealed by the number of choices required to select the correct choice. It can be transformed into the d' measure of signal detection theory (SDT) if the standard assumptions of this theory hold, but not in general. Two experiments on the recognition of words-in-noise are described. In the first there were 3, 5, 8 or 16 choices. They were typed on a card which was displayed to the listener during the presentation of a word. On both measures 5-, 8- and 16-choice recognition did not differ significantly, while 3-choice recognition was slightly, but significantly, superior. The second experiment concerned the effect of delaying the display of the card until 2 sec. after presentation. No evidence for an effect was obtained. In both experiments, there was suggestive but not conclusive evidence that the d' measure tended to overestimate recognition efficiency.  相似文献   
864.
Book reviews     
Encyclopaedia of Linguistics, Information and Control. Edited by A. R. Meetham with the assistance of R. A. Hudson. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 1969. Pp. xiv + 718. 300s.

The Biological Basis of Personality. By H. J. Eysenck. Springfield: Thomas. 1968. Pp. xv + 399. $18.50.

Scientific Basis of Dvug Dependence. Biological Council Symposium. Edited by H. Steinberg. London: Churchill. 1969. Pp. 429. 100s.

Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 2. Edited by John P. Hill. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. 1969. Pp. viii + 172.

Memory and Attention: an introduction to human information processes. By Donald A. Norman. Chichester, Sussex: John Wiley. 1969. Pp. ix + 201. 84s. (cloth); 44s. (paper). Verbal Learning and Memory: selected readings. By Leo Postman and Geoffrey Keppel. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. 1969. Pp. 501. 12s.

Handbuch der Kinderheilkunde. Edited by H. Opitz and F. Schmidt. Rand VIII/I: Neurologie-Psychologic-Psychiatrie. Redigiert von F. Schmidt und H. Asperger. Berlin: Springer Verlag. Pp. 1060. Price DM385, S96.25.

Digital Computing. By R. S. Lehman and D. E. Bailey. London: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 1969. Pp. xxiv + 303. 79s.

Real-Time Computers. By W. R. Uttal. New York: Harper & Row. 1968. Pp. xiv + 338. 93s.  相似文献   
865.
866.
867.
Pigeons were exposed to four different schedules of food reinforcement that arranged a fixed minimum time interval between reinforcements (60 sec or 300 sec). The first was a standard fixed-interval schedule. The second was a schedule in which food was presented automatically at the end of the fixed time interval as long as a response had occurred earlier. The third and fourth schedules were identical to the first two except that the first response after reinforcement changed the color on the key. When the schedule required a peck after the interval elapsed, the response pattern consisted of a pause after reinforcement followed by responding at a high rate until reinforcement. When a response was not required after the termination of the interval, the pattern consisted of a pause after reinforcement, followed by responses and then by a subsequent pause until reinforcement. Having the first response after reinforcement change the color on the key had little effect on performance. Post-reinforcement pause duration varied with the minimum interreinforcement interval but was unaffected by whether or not a response was required after the interval elapsed.  相似文献   
868.
Three experiments were conducted to investigate the theoretical reduction of rate and duration of reinforcement to their product, rate of reinforcement-time, under concurrent chain schedules. In Exp. I, rate of reinforcement-time was varied by varying rate of reinforcement delivery, holding duration of reinforcement availability constant; in Exp. II, rate of reinforcement-time was varied by holding rate of reinforcement delivery constant and varying duration of reinforcement availability; in Exp. III, rate of reinforcement-time was held constant by varying both rate and duration of reinforcement simultaneously and inversely. For all three experiments, both relative rate of responding and relative time spent in the initial link were found to match approximately the relative rate of reinforcement-time arranged in the terminal link. These data were interpreted as support for the notion that rate and duration of reinforcement may be functionally equivalent and reducible to a single variable, rate of reinforcement-time.  相似文献   
869.
Some punishing effects of response-force   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
The present experiment explored the punishing effect of different response-force requirements by means of a two-operant design analogous to a two-component chain schedule. The first component of the chain required a lever pull through 0.25 in. (0.64 cm) at 1 lb (4.45 N) of force. The second component required a lever pull through an additional 0.75 in. (1.90 cm) with the force varied between sessions from 1 lb to 50 lb (4.45 N to 223 N). Completion of the second component of the chain was reinforced after variable intervals averaging 1 min. The average rate of first-component response decreased as the force requirement for second-component responses was increased. This rate reduction did not appear to be due to increased response duration, “fatigue”, or differing rates of reinforcement. If the force requirement for the second-component response is viewed as a consequence for the first-component response, then the results of the experiment show that a high force requirement is a punisher.  相似文献   
870.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号