首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   58232篇
  免费   1533篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2020年   459篇
  2019年   571篇
  2018年   4035篇
  2017年   3447篇
  2016年   2941篇
  2015年   774篇
  2014年   784篇
  2013年   3152篇
  2012年   1826篇
  2011年   3578篇
  2010年   3203篇
  2009年   2215篇
  2008年   2806篇
  2007年   3255篇
  2006年   1098篇
  2005年   1143篇
  2004年   1051篇
  2003年   930篇
  2002年   914篇
  2001年   1215篇
  2000年   1201篇
  1999年   921篇
  1998年   417篇
  1997年   406篇
  1996年   371篇
  1995年   360篇
  1993年   359篇
  1992年   764篇
  1991年   666篇
  1990年   731篇
  1989年   622篇
  1988年   661篇
  1987年   616篇
  1986年   612篇
  1985年   559篇
  1984年   513篇
  1983年   495篇
  1979年   591篇
  1978年   404篇
  1977年   353篇
  1975年   462篇
  1974年   510篇
  1973年   537篇
  1972年   409篇
  1971年   404篇
  1970年   360篇
  1969年   417篇
  1968年   491篇
  1967年   442篇
  1966年   366篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
53.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号