首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2343篇
  免费   137篇
  国内免费   61篇
  2023年   18篇
  2022年   21篇
  2021年   41篇
  2020年   62篇
  2019年   52篇
  2018年   76篇
  2017年   75篇
  2016年   85篇
  2015年   51篇
  2014年   74篇
  2013年   260篇
  2012年   130篇
  2011年   117篇
  2010年   84篇
  2009年   77篇
  2008年   98篇
  2007年   107篇
  2006年   85篇
  2005年   97篇
  2004年   81篇
  2003年   49篇
  2002年   67篇
  2001年   39篇
  2000年   44篇
  1999年   47篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   27篇
  1996年   21篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   23篇
  1993年   23篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   25篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   21篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   28篇
  1986年   21篇
  1985年   19篇
  1984年   18篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   18篇
  1979年   20篇
  1978年   17篇
  1974年   19篇
  1973年   17篇
  1972年   14篇
  1971年   16篇
  1967年   13篇
排序方式: 共有2541条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
正常和弱视者立体和形状视觉感知时间的差异   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
在速示器,以随机点立体图对为刺激,测量了正常和弱视者的立体视和形状视感知时间。实验表明:正常被试需要大约100ms感知深度,110ms或更长时间感知形状。这二种感知时间与刺激视差的种类和大小无关。弱视者需要比正常人较长的时间达到立体和形状的视感知。这二种感知时间的差别,弱视者也大于正常人。视锐度和立体视范围均恢复到正常的弱视者,以上二种感知时间,可以达到正常人范围,也可能显著地大于正常人。  相似文献   
962.
Two experiments with rats as subjects are described, which control for social position, i.e., relative dominance/submission, in an appetitive social learning-performance setting. The results indicate that animals that perform quite effectively when alone exhibit significantly reduced levels of responding when placed into a social environment. The severity of the response decrement is, at least in part, a function of the relative social position of the subjects involved. A dominant male made few responses when paired with another dominant male. Yet, a dominant subject made even fewer responses when paired with a submissive subject, which bar pressed at approximately half the individual level. These findings are interpreted as suggesting that social position, with its accompanying characteristic form of aggression, is an important determinant of performance in a social learning environment.  相似文献   
963.
In the first experiment rats experienced large or small magnitude of negative reinforcement (shock reduction) in a straight alley. Half of the subjects in each magnitude group received continuous reinforcement, and the other half received a 50% partial reinforcement schedule (nonreinforcement consisting of no shock reduction in the goal box). In extinction the groups were ordered: large partial > small-partial > small-continuous > large-continuous. In the second experiment rats received large, small and nonreinforcement in various sequences using the runway-negative reinforcement procedure and were ordered: SNL>LNL>SNL>LNS in resistance to extinction (letters represent the magnitudes in the sequence experienced in acquisition). The results of these experiments indicate a commality between positive and negative reinforcement with respect to behavioral phenomena and theoretical accounts of those phenomena.  相似文献   
964.
965.
Four groups of 40 Ss were given transfer tests following 2.4, 6, or 8 14-trial blocks of paired-associate training with unequal stimulus frequencies and then retested after a further training period in which the same compound stimuli were presented at equal frequencies. The novelty effect, in which Ss respond to new combinations of training components by giving the response associated with the least frequent component, was obtained in every transfer test. It was concluded that the novelty effect is a mode of responding that is produced by differential frequencies during early learning and which, once adopted, is retained by Ss regardless of whether the frequency differential is maintained.  相似文献   
966.
The odor-testing apparatus described consists of a circular open-field area opening into four glass tubes, each with an odor source at the far end. When a rat is introduced, each tube is blocked by a grid; when the rat has touched each grid (sampled each odor), the tubes automatically open and recording of preference behavior starts. Two measures of preference are recorded by a photocell outside each tube, the number of visits and the time spent near the odor source. A uniformity test with food odor in all tubes showed that the apparatus did not promote position bias. In two separate sensitivity tests, one with food odor and one with estrous female urine odor, male rats significantly preferred an attractive odor in one tube to odorless controls. The apparatus yields relatively sensitive and reliable odor preference determinations by ensuring sampling of each odor before data are recorded and by eliminating visual, auditory and gustatory cues.  相似文献   
967.
A computerized attempt to replicate Aronson and Carlsmith's test of expectancy theory was conducted. Generally, subjects performed consistent with their expectancy, although the effect of expectancy was strongly attentuated by an overall achievement orientation. The results contradict the hypotheses that experimenter bias, demand characteristics, or differential recall of performance mediate the effects of expectancy upon performance.  相似文献   
968.
A scale of the “psychological magnitude” of number was constructed from similarity ratings of the 45 number pairs that can be obtained from a set of 10 integers. A nonmetric analysis of these similarity ratings showed that “psychological number” was a power function of number.  相似文献   
969.
970.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号