首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   786篇
  免费   65篇
  国内免费   62篇
  913篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   28篇
  2018年   25篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   34篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   32篇
  2013年   81篇
  2012年   38篇
  2011年   48篇
  2010年   33篇
  2009年   32篇
  2008年   54篇
  2007年   45篇
  2006年   39篇
  2005年   39篇
  2004年   35篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   16篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   6篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   6篇
  1978年   8篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   3篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   2篇
排序方式: 共有913条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
Temporal form-part integration is the process whereby two discrete sets of stimuli, presented at different times, are perceived by the visual system as a single integrated percept. Dixon and Di Lollo (Cognitive Psychology 26(1):33-63, 1994) proposed a temporal correlation model that was able to quantitatively account for a number of findings related to both form-part integration tasks and partial report. The present study demonstrates a novel approach to form-part integration—the addition of a whole-field mask stimulus at the termination of the second set of stimuli. According to an extension of the correlation model, the mask stimulus should reduce the visual system’s response to the trailing display, thus increasing the relative overlap of the two displays, fostering integration. Experiment 1 supported this hypothesis, showing a maximum benefit when the mask followed the trailing display immediately, as opposed to after a delay of 60 ms. Experiment 2 showed that this same mask actually did yield worse performance when presented immediately after a single set of stimuli. The third experiment collected detailed data for a few observers over a larger number of mask delays. Taken together, these experiments provide a rare example of masking degrading a target stimulus, and yet aiding perception.  相似文献   
202.
The Computers in Teaching Initiative (CTI) Centre for Psychology has been promoting the use of computers in psychology education for the last 5 years. The advent of the Internet and ubiquitous access to the World-Wide Web has the potential to revolutionize the way the teaching and learning of psychology can be supported. This paper describes how we are using the Web to support psychology education through information dissemination, through the provision of teaching resources, and through facilities for communication.  相似文献   
203.
旦正加  嘉样 《法音》2022,(1):63-67
藏传佛教音乐是中华经典音乐的重要组成部分,是中华优秀传统文化中的一颗璀璨明珠。藏文化总体分为大五明和小五明,藏传佛教音乐艺术应归于大五明之工巧明。然而,从藏族传统文化中的工巧明分类而言,又可以分为身工巧、语工巧、意工巧。那么,藏传佛教音乐便可归入语工巧之中,世称藏传佛教六十种梵音,乃是用深如雷音、美妙悦耳、赏心可意、悠扬明晰和入耳中听的佛音五支来赞颂诸佛菩萨之殊胜功德。  相似文献   
204.
尹华站  李丹 《心理科学》2007,30(2):383-386
攀缘激活指神经元在两个先后出现事件的间隔阶段呈现出的一种缓慢爬升式“期待”激活。这种激活的强度在后一事件出现时达最高峰,以致被视为一种可能时距编码机制而成为目前时间生物学研究的一个热点问题。目前攀援激活的计时作用神经机制有三种主要观点:膜电流假说、双稳态单元假说、线性整合假说。未来研究将主要集中于探索攀缘激活的神经元突触机制,非固定间隔时距条件下攀缘激活的计时机制及攀缘激活、运动准备和脉冲累加之间的关系。  相似文献   
205.
童年期遗忘指人们无法回忆起生命最初几年的事情。最早记忆是童年期遗忘开始消退和自传体记忆发生的标志,诸多研究表明成人的最早记忆一般介于3~4岁间,儿童有更早的最早记忆。最早记忆的诱发方法,包括单一最早记忆法、线索词法、穷举法和生命全程法;最早记忆的评估方法,包括自陈式的记忆现象学问卷测量,以及针对记忆叙述内容的编码分析。未来研究要加强纵向研究,追踪最早记忆随年龄而发生的变化;展开特殊人群的研究;完善测量工具;探究早期记忆保持和遗忘的机制。  相似文献   
206.
王婷  王丹  张积家  崔健爱 《心理学报》2017,(11):1392-1403
在中国云南的景颇族中,相当多的家庭是由不同支系的人组成的,家庭成员交际时存在着"各说各话"的现象。"各说各话"对讲话者的执行功能有无影响?采用抑制控制、注意转换和记忆刷新任务测量各说各话和非各说各话的景颇族大学生的抑制能力、转换能力和刷新能力。结果表明,各说各话与非各说各话的景颇族大学生在色词干扰和数字转换中差异显著,各说各话者的Stroop效应量小,停止信号的反应时短。各说各话与非各说各话的景颇族大学生在数字转换和图形转换中差异显著,各说各话者反应快,转换代价小。各说各话与非各说各话的景颇族大学生在活动记忆刷新与色点位置刷新任务中差异不显著。这表明,各说各话者比非各说各话者在抑制能力与转换能力上具有优势。语言经验对执行功能的影响具有功能特异性。  相似文献   
207.
In this paper I claim that Quinean naturalist accounts of science, that deny that there are any a priori statements in scientific frameworks, cannot account for the foundational role of certain classes of statements in scientific practice. In this I follow Michael Friedman who claims that certain a priori statements must be presupposed in order to formulate empirical hypotheses. I also show that Friedman’s account, in spite of his claims to the contrary, is compatible with a type of non‐Quinean naturalism that I sketch. Finally I also show that Friedman’s account needs amending because it cannot provide a rational account of theory change. I accomplish this by arguing for a structural realist view of theory change. I show how this view fits well with an account like Friedman’s and helps it deal with the problem of theory change and in retaining its superiority over Quinean naturalism.  相似文献   
208.
Among African countries, Botswana stands out for achieving lasting political and economic stability, which has enabled the government to develop a strong system of educational and social services for its people. Development of professional counseling in the country has occurred both through targeted efforts to provide a strong system of guidance services in schools and in response to the impact of the HIV/AIDS epidemic. This article reviews the development of professional counseling in Botswana and highlights trends likely to shape its future.  相似文献   
209.
Training transfer is crucial for workplace effectiveness, yet some of its social support antecedents are not well investigated. We examine the extent to which two forms of social support (from the organization and the supervisor) predict training transfer and propose several related processes explaining these influences. First, social support enhances training self‐efficacy, increases trainees' mastery (learning) goal orientation, and their motivation to transfer. In turn, we propose that these individual factors are positively related to training‐related cognitions. We test and confirm our model in a longitudinal study based on a sample of 111 employees. The results contribute to a better understanding of organization‐ and supervisor‐based social support as predictors of transfer, and clarify several important related mechanisms.  相似文献   
210.
Major depressive disorder aggregates within families, although the mechanisms of transfer across generations are not well understood. In light of converging biological and behavioral evidence that depressive symptoms are associated with impaired reward processing, we examined whether adolescent girls with a parental history of depression would also exhibit abnormal reward sensitivity. We performed a negative mood induction and then recorded the feedback negativity, a neural index of reward processing, while individuals completed a gambling task. High-risk adolescents reported greater sadness following the mood induction compared to low-risk adolescents. Among the high-risk group, sadness was strongly associated with a blunted feedback negativity, even after controlling for baseline mood and trait neuroticism. This suggests that high-risk adolescents are more reactive to negative stimuli, which significantly alter neural sensitivity to monetary gains and losses. The feedback negativity might be used to identify information processing abnormalities in high-risk populations prior to the onset of a major depressive episode.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号