首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1176篇
  免费   80篇
  国内免费   61篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   31篇
  2019年   34篇
  2018年   34篇
  2017年   33篇
  2016年   44篇
  2015年   26篇
  2014年   39篇
  2013年   124篇
  2012年   47篇
  2011年   56篇
  2010年   44篇
  2009年   40篇
  2008年   62篇
  2007年   56篇
  2006年   48篇
  2005年   47篇
  2004年   44篇
  2003年   28篇
  2002年   41篇
  2001年   17篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   17篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   15篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   8篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   13篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   8篇
  1972年   8篇
  1971年   9篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   8篇
  1950年   8篇
排序方式: 共有1317条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
Algorithms for approximate Bayesian inference, such as those based on sampling (i.e., Monte Carlo methods), provide a natural source of models of how people may deal with uncertainty with limited cognitive resources. Here, we consider the idea that individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) may be usefully modeled in terms of the number of samples, or “particles,” available to perform inference. To test this idea, we focus on two recent experiments that report positive associations between WMC and two distinct aspects of categorization performance: the ability to learn novel categories, and the ability to switch between different categorization strategies (“knowledge restructuring”). In favor of the idea of modeling WMC as a number of particles, we show that a single model can reproduce both experimental results by varying the number of particles—increasing the number of particles leads to both faster category learning and improved strategy‐switching. Furthermore, when we fit the model to individual participants, we found a positive association between WMC and best‐fit number of particles for strategy switching. However, no association between WMC and best‐fit number of particles was found for category learning. These results are discussed in the context of the general challenge of disentangling the contributions of different potential sources of behavioral variability.  相似文献   
142.
143.
Pragmatics     
Dan Sperber 《Cognition》1981,10(1-3):281-286
  相似文献   
144.
145.
146.
The pro‐life paradox, as I call it, begins with a single claim endorsed by many American Christians: infants and young children are innocent in the sight of God because they cannot yet take responsibility for their spiritual well‐being. With this in mind, I argue that pro‐life believers have unwittingly fallen victim to a theological paradox in which their attempts to save the earthly lives of unborn children make it theoretically possible for said children to die an eternal death. On the one hand, many Christians trust in an eventual spiritual reckoning where God will separate the “sheep” from the “goats” (see Matthew 25:31–46), ushering the former into heaven while damning the latter to hell. However, those who cannot yet repent and seek salvation are not blamed for their spiritual failings. If they die, they go to heaven because they are too young and intellectually immature to know any better. But if dead children are spiritually blameless, then abortion practitioners have perversely and paradoxically saved millions of unborn souls by removing human volition (and thus damnation itself) from the equation and by making it possible for the unborn to experience the joys of heaven without the temptations of earth.  相似文献   
147.
A modification of the Grason-Stadler Series 829 D&E Electronic Switches that enables simultaneous cross-channel presentation of auditory signals at the two ears is described.  相似文献   
148.
149.
150.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号