首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   21791篇
  免费   97篇
  国内免费   61篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   22篇
  2020年   39篇
  2019年   38篇
  2018年   3510篇
  2017年   2842篇
  2016年   2283篇
  2015年   217篇
  2014年   112篇
  2013年   242篇
  2012年   615篇
  2011年   2447篇
  2010年   2555篇
  2009年   1509篇
  2008年   1767篇
  2007年   2232篇
  2006年   80篇
  2005年   268篇
  2004年   213篇
  2003年   148篇
  2002年   105篇
  2001年   46篇
  2000年   79篇
  1999年   35篇
  1998年   37篇
  1997年   44篇
  1996年   21篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   13篇
  1990年   27篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   14篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   29篇
  1977年   16篇
  1976年   12篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   14篇
  1973年   13篇
  1972年   11篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Forty-four juvenile rhesus, 30 stumptailed, and 6 pigtailed monkeys were tested individually and in stable quadrads for time to contact slightly and highly novel objects. When peer-group tested, dominant monkeys were the first to contact the slightly novel but not the highly novel objects, unlike preferences found when they were tested individually. A role analysis revealed better contact-order prediction, most groups having their habitual first contactor. When this contactor was overtly punished or covertly trained to avoid the object, group response was altered. The use of the termrole is discussed in detail, concluding that a role involves a particular individual who is expected to interact with others while in certain groups to complete some beneficial function.  相似文献   
72.
It is shown that lexical decision times to strong associates with an associative strength of approximately 40% are facilitated relative to targets following a neutral prime, “blank,” whereas very weak associates with an associative strength of less then 3% are neither facilitated nor inhibited. It is also shown that relative to the “blank” baseline time, a row of crosses inhibits processing of the following target. The latter finding has implications for earlier studies that have used crosses as a neutral prime. In these studies, facilitation effects have been overestimated and inhibition effects have been underestimated. Neely 1976 has proposed a predict-and-match strategy according to which subjects are assumed to predict one or more targets from the prime and to match the actual target onto the predicted targets. A part of this theory is not supported by the present data. The results are discussed in terms of the two-process theory of expectancy (Posner & Snyder, 1975). They are also considered in the light of a recent theory by Becker (1980). As an alternative interpretation of part of the reported data, a coherence assumption by the subjects about all reading materials is introduced.  相似文献   
73.
A growing body of research suggests that low Mental Age (MA) autistic and retarded children show a unique stimulus control deficit, one that may cause many or most of their behavioral deficiencies. This problem, stimulus overselectivity, is evidenced when a child responds only to a restricted portion of the stimulus environment when compared with normal children. The purpose of this study was to assess whether this overselectivity is general across situations or whether it is restricted to certain stimulus/task conditions. Eight autistic children, who evidenced overselectivity on a preassessment task, and 8 normal children with similar MA levels participated. All children were trained on 3 tasks to determine if overselectivity varied as a function of different stimulus conditions. Each of the 3 tasks involved training a child to respond to (i.e., touch) a card containing a circle (S +) and to avoid a blank (S ?) card. In each case, the circle comprised a series of dots. The difference between the 3 circles (tasks) was the distance between the successive dots making up each circle. Also, in the minimal separation condition the dots were smaller in size and greater in number than in the larger separation conditions. Of concern was whether autistic children learned about the gestalt (i.e., the circle), which required attention to multiple cues, or whether children would overselect and respond to the dots. The results showed that (1) stimulus overselectivity was found not to be a generalized deficit in autistic subjects; instead, it varied as a function of the stimulus variables; and (2) the stimulus variables manipulated in this study similarly influenced the responding of both normal and autistic children. The implications of these data for a theory of overselectivity are discussed.  相似文献   
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号