全文获取类型
收费全文 | 373篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
380篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 44篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有380条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
321.
Although hundreds of studies have found a positive relationship between self-efficacy and performance, several studies have found a negative relationship when the analysis is done across time (repeated measures) rather than across individuals. W. T. Powers (1991) predicted this negative relationship based on perceptual control theory. Here, 2 studies are presented to (a) confirm the causal role of self-efficacy and (b) substantiate the explanation. In Study 1, self-efficacy was manipulated for 43 of 87 undergraduates on an analytic game. The manipulation was negatively related to performance on the next trial. In Study 2, 104 undergraduates played the analytic game and reported self-efficacy between each game and confidence in the degree to which they had assessed previous feedback. As expected, self-efficacy led to overconfidence and hence increased the likelihood of committing logic errors during the game. 相似文献
322.
323.
Roscoe A. Dykman Philip C. Loizou Peggy T. Ackerman Patrick H. Casey W. Brian McPherson 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2000,35(4):284-297
Sixty-five subjects, ages 8 to 12, participated in a visual electrophysiological study. Twenty-two of the subjects had received
a diagnosis of nonorganic failure-to-thrive (FTT) before the age of three. The remaining 43 subjects had no history of FTT
and served as Controls. IQs were obtained with the abbreviated WISC-III, and the Controls were split into two groups, LO IQ
and HI IQ, to provide a LO IQ Control group with an average IQ equivalent to the FTT group. Event-related brain potentials
(ERPs) were recorded from five scalp locations during a cued continuous performance task (CPT). Subjects had to press a button
every time they saw the letter “X” following the letter “A” (50 targets out of 400 stimuli).
During the CPT, the FTT subjects made marginally more errors of omission to targets than the LO IQ Control group and significantly
more errors of omission than the HI IQ Control subjects. The groups did not differ significantly on errors of commission (false
alarms) or reaction times to targets. ERP averages revealed a group difference in amplitude in a late slow wave for the 50
non-X stimuli (false targets) that followed the letter A. This difference was greatest over frontal sites, where the FTT group
had a more negative going slow wave than each control group. Late frontal negativity to No Go stimuli has been linked with
post-decisional processing, notably in young children. Thus, the FTT subjects may have less efficient inhibitory processes,
reflected by additional late frontal activation. *** DIRECT SUPPORT *** A31BB040 00006 相似文献
324.
Jesse F. Casey 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》1981,11(2):67-74
ABSTRACT: An episode of total paralysis (from medication) with full consciousness is described and its relation to death terror is discussed. 相似文献
325.
Amori Yee Mikami Matthew D. Lerner Marissa Swaim Griggs Alison McGrath Casey D. Calhoun 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2010,38(6):737-749
We report findings from a pilot intervention that trained parents to be “friendship coaches” for their children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD). Parents of 62 children with ADHD (ages 6–10; 68% male) were randomly assigned to receive the parental friendship
coaching (PFC) intervention, or to be in a no-treatment control group. Families of 62 children without ADHD were included
as normative comparisons. PFC was administered in eight, 90-minute sessions to parents; there was no child treatment component.
Parents were taught to arrange a social context in which their children were optimally likely to develop good peer relationships.
Receipt of PFC predicted improvements in children’s social skills and friendship quality on playdates as reported by parents,
and peer acceptance and rejection as reported by teachers unaware of treatment status. PFC also predicted increases in observed
parental facilitation and corrective feedback, and reductions in criticism during the child’s peer interaction, which mediated
the improvements in children’s peer relationships. However, no effects for PFC were found on the number of playdates hosted
or on teacher report of child social skills. Findings lend initial support to a treatment model that targets parental behaviors
to address children’s peer problems. 相似文献
326.
327.
Casey Rebecca Johnson 《International Journal of Philosophical Studies》2015,23(3):356-375
AbstractI can, given the right conditions, transmit my knowledge to you by telling you some information. If I know the time, and if all goes well, I can bring it about that you know it too. If conditions are right, all I have to do is assert to you what time it is. Paradigmatically, speakers use assertions to transmit what they know to their hearers. Clearly, assertion and testimony are tightly connected. The nature of this connection, however, is not so clear. According to many accounts, assertion has an epistemic constitutive norm. This norm appears to be able to account for some important features of testimony: first, testimonial knowledge transmission, second, the reliability of testimony, and third, the epistemic rights exchanged in cases of testimony. In this paper, however, I argue against this apparent ability. The constitutive norm of assertion, I argue, plays no role in accounts of testimonial knowledge transmission, or of the epistemic rights that testimony confers. This is especially clear when we consider the general norms to which we’re held. Epistemological accounts of testimony can and should, therefore, avoid the difficult debate over the constitutive norm of assertion. 相似文献
328.
329.
The human brain undergoes significant changes in both its structural architecture and functional organization across the life span. Advances in neuroimaging techniques over the past decade have allowed us to track these changes safely in the human in vivo. We review the imaging literature on the neurobiology of cognitive development, focusing specifically on cognitive task-dependent changes observed in brain physiology and anatomy across childhood and adolescence. The findings suggest that cortical function becomes fine-tuned with development. Brain regions associated with more basic functions such as sensory and motor processes mature first, followed by association areas involved in top-down control of behavior. 相似文献
330.
Wong AC Gauthier I Woroch B DeBuse C Curran T 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2005,5(3):306-318
Expertise with print is likely to optimize visual processes for recognizing characters of a familiar writing system. Although
brain activations have been identified for words and letter strings in contrast with other stimuli, relatively little work
has focused on the neural basis of single-letter perception. English readers and Chinese-English bilinguals participated in
an ERP study and performed a 1-back identity judgment on Roman letters, Chinese characters, pseudofonts, and their string
versions. The Chinese-English bilinguals showed an enhanced N170 for both Roman letters and Chinese characters relative to
pseudofonts. For the non-Chinese readers, the N170 amplitude was larger for Roman letters relative to Chinese characters and
pseudofonts. Our results suggest that changes in relatively early visual processes underlie expert letter perception. 相似文献