全文获取类型
收费全文 | 137篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
141篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 4篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 13篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 7篇 |
2010年 | 1篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 11篇 |
2007年 | 3篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有141条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
The Associates programme at the University of East Anglia is designed as an opportunity for self-development and as a testing ground for those who want to explore their own helping abilities. The structure and content of the programme are described; some examples are provided of the range of follow-up activities which have arisen from it; and attention is given to the potential of the programme as a contribution to the development of the institution as a community. 相似文献
122.
123.
124.
Russell Re Manning 《International Journal of Systematic Theology》2013,15(4):437-452
Throughout his theological career Paul Tillich (1886–1965) was fundamentally concerned with the question of the religious meaning of culture. The answers that he gave – initially in the revolutionary ferment of 1920s–1930s Germany and then again in the brave new world of post‐World War II America – were profound and far‐reaching in their importance for twentieth‐century theology. This article will explore the development of Tillich's interpretations of the religious meaning of culture and apply his analyses to our contemporary religious and cultural situation by suggesting a trinitarian enlargement of Tillich's concept of theonomous metaphysics towards a multidimensional theology of culture. 相似文献
125.
Studied experimentally the influence of norms and sex of subjects on aggressive behaviour in same sex dyads. Two hypotheses were tested: (1) subjects will react aggressively to an unpleasant state of affairs, if they interpret it as being the result of violation of a norm on the part of another; (2) female subjects will display more aggression than male subjects under conditions of repeated provocation while male subjects will be more aggressive under conditions of infrequent or no provocation. In a 2 × 2 × 2 complete factorial design (norm violation versus norm enforcement; male versus female; low versus high reward for performance) 20 same sex pairs of students performed alternatively a sensory-motor task (victim) and a shock delivery task (aggressor). As predicted, subjects who consider other's behaviour to be a norm violation aggress more often (p <.0001). A significant interaction between sex of subject and norm violation is found in support for the second hypothesis (p <.05). It is concluded that positions grounded on the S-R paradigm are misleading for the understanding of sex differences in aggression. 相似文献
126.
Da Cunha C Wietzikoski S Wietzikoski EC Silva MH Chandler J Ferro MM Andreatini R Canteras NS 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2007,87(4):451-463
The bilateral intranigral infusion of 1 micromol 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) in adult male Wistar rats caused a specific and partial loss of substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) dopamine neurons, a partial depletion of striatal dopamine, and a deficit to learn the intra-maze cued version of the Morris water maze. Pre-training the SNc rats in the spatial version of the water maze or simply maintaining the animals on the water maze platform reversed this deficit. This improvement was even observed when the order of the extra-maze cues presented to the rats during pre-training of the spatial version was changed during training of the intra-maze cued version. However, this deficit was not reversed either by maintaining the animals on the platform if the spatial cues were surrounded and covered with a curtain or by swimming sessions in the maze without the escape platform and the curtain. These findings suggest that none of the following elements alone, learned during the spatial task pre-training, could help SNc rats learn the intra-maze cued task: improvement of swimming skills or knowledge of the existence of the escape platform; distance between the platform and the border of the pool; location of a particular extra-maze cue; relations among extra-maze cues. However, the simultaneous presence of the escape platform and extra-maze cues (irrespective of their relational configuration) during the pre-training sessions proved to be necessary for this improving effect to occur. 相似文献
127.
Attention to low- and high-spatial frequencies in categorizing facial identities, emotions and gender in children with autism 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Deruelle C Rondan C Salle-Collemiche X Bastard-Rosset D Da Fonséca D 《Brain and cognition》2008,66(2):115-123
This study was aimed at investigating face categorization strategies in children with autistic spectrum disorders (ASD). Performance of 17 children with ASD was compared to that of 17 control children in a face-matching task, including hybrid faces (composed of two overlapping faces of different spatial bandwidths) and either low- or high-pass filtered faces. Participants were asked to match faces on the basis of identity, emotion or gender. Results revealed that children with ASD used the same strategies as controls when matching faces by gender. By contrast, in the identity and the emotion conditions, children with ASD showed a high-pass bias (i.e., preference for local information), contrary to controls. Consistent with previous studies on autism, these findings suggest that children with ASD do use atypical (local-oriented) strategies to process faces. 相似文献
128.
J A Da Silva 《The American journal of psychology》1985,98(1):119-144
Scales for perceived egocentric distance produced by three psychophysical methods in four ranges of distances were compared. It was found that (a) the exponents produced by ratio and fractionation methods are in good agreement; (b) the exponents of both these methods were larger than those produced by magnitude estimation; (c) an increase in range of distance was associated with a decrease in exponent, but this diminution seems to interact with the method used; (d) for all the psychophysical methods used, there was large variability in the individual exponent; (e) the exponent was smaller than 1.0 for approximately 78% of the pooled sample, with all adult observers (N = 612) considered; and (f) an arithmetic mean exponent equal to 0.90 represents fairly all the results obtained. 相似文献
129.
高校合并:医科院校的发展目标与制度创新 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
贺达仁 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2001,22(10):16-19
合并后的高校其发展战略目标是创办综合性、研究型大学。高校的核心职能是民族创新的智力引擎,为实现21世纪中华民族的复兴,合并后高校的中心工作就是创新。当前合并后高校的工作重点是围绕发展战略目标,实现在学科建设与重组、机构改革与设置、人才培养与使用、科研项目与市场以及管理功能等方面的制度创新。 相似文献
130.
Jairo José Da Silva 《Axiomathes》2002,13(2):107-126
In this paper I argue for the view that the axioms of ZF are analytic truths of a particular concept of set. By this I mean
that these axioms are true by virtue only of the meaning attached to this concept, and, moreover, can be derived from it.
Although I assume that the object of ZF is a concept of set, I refrain from asserting either its independent existence, or
its dependence on subjectivity. All I presuppose is that this concept is given to us with a certain sense as the objective
focus of a ”phenomenologically reduced“ intentional experience.
The concept of set that ZF describes, I claim, is that of a multiplicity of coexisting elements that can, as a consequence,
be a member of another multiplicity. A set is conceived as a quantitatively determined collection of objects that is, by necessity,
ontologically dependent on its elements, which, on the other hand, must exist independently of it. A close scrutiny of the
essential characters of this conception seems to be sufficient to ground the set-theoretic hierarchy and the axioms of ZF.
This revised version was published online in August 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献