全文获取类型
收费全文 | 761篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
764篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 12篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 26篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 27篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 26篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 26篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 12篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有764条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
521.
522.
While attributional processes are traditionally regarded as intrapersonal phenomena, attributional statements are a common feature of interpersonal communication. This study sought to explore the nature of attributions that are used tactically in conversation. Among other tasks, participants were asked to describe conversations in which causal statements had been posited for some “ulterior” purpose—perhaps to evoke an emotion, obtain a behavior, or receive some sort of compliment. Results revealed that the most commonly cited tactical attributions included excuses, justifications, and affiliation moves. Nontactical attributions, in contrast, rarely included affiliation moves. Attributions that were used tactically also tended to focus more frequently on positive events, behaviors, or qualities and tended to emphasize interpersonal, stable sources more often than nontactical attributions. The most frequently perceived goal for tactical attributions was to obtain an object or behavior. Implications for an extension of the literature on accounts are discussed. 相似文献
523.
DAVID C. DEVONIS 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》2012,48(1):16-39
The psychologist Timothy Leary (1920–1996), an iconic cultural figure in the United States in the 1960s and afterward, has received comparatively scant attention in the history of psychology. This may be due to perceptions that, after a major career shift centering around his experimentation with psychedelic substances and his subsequent dismissal from Harvard in 1963, Leary parted company with the field. While there are several good reasons to adopt this view, examination of his entire career as well as his intellectual ancestry reveals unacknowledged continuities, suggesting that a more prominent place be accorded to him in the history of psychology, as well as to the challenges he poses. 相似文献
524.
Aristotle on the Homonymy of Being 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
DAVID PITT 《Philosophy and phenomenological research》2004,69(1):1-36
A number of philosophers endorse, without argument, the view that there's something it's like consciously to think that p , which is distinct from what it's like consciously to think that q . This thesis, if true, would have important consequences for philosophy of mind and cognitive science. In this paper I offer two arguments for it.
The first argument claims it would be impossible introspectively to distinguish conscious thoughts with respect to their content if there weren't something it's like to think them. This argument is defended against several objections.
The second argument uses what I call "minimal pair" experiences—sentences read without and with understanding—to induce in the reader an experience of the kind I claim exists. Further objections are considered and rebutted. 相似文献
The first argument claims it would be impossible introspectively to distinguish conscious thoughts with respect to their content if there weren't something it's like to think them. This argument is defended against several objections.
The second argument uses what I call "minimal pair" experiences—sentences read without and with understanding—to induce in the reader an experience of the kind I claim exists. Further objections are considered and rebutted. 相似文献
525.
526.
527.
DAVID ANTONIONI 《Personnel Psychology》1994,47(2):349-356
Do subordinates feel and respond differently to upward appraisal procedures depending on whether they are accountable or anonymous? Accountability requires subordinates to identify themselves on the upward appraisal questionnaires they complete. In an experimental field study, 38 managers and their subordinates from an insurance company were randomly assigned to use one of the appraisal procedures. As hypothesized, managers who received feedback from specific individuals in the accountability procedure viewed the upward appraisal process more positively than did managers in the anonymity procedure. However, subordinates felt more comfortable giving anonymous responses. As a result, subordinates who used the accountability procedure rated their managers significantly higher than the subordinates who responded anonymously. Therefore, the results of this study suggest that an upward appraisal procedure in which individuals are accountable for their responses may produce inflated ratings of managers' performance. Further research should compare the accountability and anonymous upward appraisal procedures to determine which procedure produces more accurate ratings. 相似文献
528.
THE EFFECTS OF APPLICANTs' REACTIONS TO COGNITIVE ABILITY TESTS AND AN ASSESSMENT CENTER 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
THERESE HOFF MACAN MARCIA J. AVEDON MATTHEW PAESE DAVID E. SMITH 《Personnel Psychology》1994,47(4):715-738
Manufacturing applicants' perceptions of two selection devices were examined. In Study 1, applicants ( n = 3,984) completed cognitive ability tests and a survey of reactions. In Study 2, a subset of applicants from Study 1 ( n = 194) participated in an assessment center and completed the survey. Applicants reacted favorably to the procedures but viewed the assessment center as more face valid than the cognitive tests. Applicants who perceived the selection techniques more favorably were also more satisfied with the selection process, the job, and the organization. Although applicants' perceptions of the procedures were related to job acceptance intentions, applicants' liking of the job and organization explained the largest unique variance. In future studies, applicants' job acceptance intentions and attitudes toward the job and organization should be assessed before and after administration of selection devices; not controlling for prior impressions resulted in overestimation of the contribution of applicants' perceptions of selection procedures. 相似文献
529.
This article examines a comprehensive model of information seeking (CMIS) resulting from the synthesis of three theoretical research streams: the health belief model, uses and gratifications research, and a model of media exposure and appraisal. The model postulates that four health-related factors—demographics, direct experience, salience, and beliefs—determine two information carrier factors—perceptions of information carrier characteristics and utility—which, in turn, determine information-seeking actions. A sample of 366 adult women drawn from a medium-size midwestern city was surveyed by telephone on the issue of mammography use and cancer-related information seeking. The model resulted in an excellent fit to the data received. However, the values of the paths associated with the health-related factors were extremely low, suggesting that models of information seeking from mass media, such as magazines, should focus on purely communicative factors. These results are discussed in terms of future research and the pragmatic benefits of this theoretical framework. 相似文献
530.