全文获取类型
收费全文 | 87574篇 |
免费 | 3398篇 |
国内免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
90999篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 1048篇 |
2018年 | 1431篇 |
2017年 | 1423篇 |
2016年 | 1552篇 |
2015年 | 1116篇 |
2014年 | 1343篇 |
2013年 | 7294篇 |
2012年 | 2434篇 |
2011年 | 2607篇 |
2010年 | 1633篇 |
2009年 | 1601篇 |
2008年 | 2373篇 |
2007年 | 2305篇 |
2006年 | 2125篇 |
2005年 | 1848篇 |
2004年 | 1730篇 |
2003年 | 1681篇 |
2002年 | 1759篇 |
2001年 | 2762篇 |
2000年 | 2686篇 |
1999年 | 2065篇 |
1998年 | 974篇 |
1997年 | 889篇 |
1996年 | 1007篇 |
1995年 | 932篇 |
1993年 | 898篇 |
1992年 | 1878篇 |
1991年 | 1724篇 |
1990年 | 1710篇 |
1989年 | 1548篇 |
1988年 | 1512篇 |
1987年 | 1495篇 |
1986年 | 1527篇 |
1985年 | 1545篇 |
1984年 | 1341篇 |
1983年 | 1178篇 |
1982年 | 884篇 |
1981年 | 893篇 |
1979年 | 1351篇 |
1978年 | 993篇 |
1975年 | 1174篇 |
1974年 | 1213篇 |
1973年 | 1333篇 |
1972年 | 1200篇 |
1971年 | 1079篇 |
1970年 | 985篇 |
1969年 | 993篇 |
1968年 | 1250篇 |
1967年 | 1097篇 |
1966年 | 997篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
22.
23.
D. THOMAS PORTER 《人类交流研究》1981,8(1):58-71
Since 1970 about 80% of the research on oral communication apprehension has used one of two versions of McCroskey's Personal Report of Communication Apprehension. The original 20-item PRCA was introduced for the purpose of measuring apprehension beyond the traditional public speaking context. Although the PRCA-20 is reliable, some serious criticism of it has centered on its ability to tap interpersonal behavior outcomes. In an attempt to bolster its interpersonal applicability, five “interpersonally-oriented” items were added to form the PRCA-25. The studies reported herein demonstrate that both versions fail to possess sufficient reliable variance to be applicable to contexts other than public settings. In addition, it was found that the interpersonal items are a separate component of the PRCA, and as such, do not justify being treated equal in terms of additivity and, therefore, reduce the construct integrity of the PRCA. 相似文献
24.
Michael J. Kelley 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1986,38(1):97-110
The study supplies further evidence that non-associative effects and temporal-spatial similarities between certain combinations of cue and consequence cannot explain all instances of stimulus-reinforcer interactions. Pigeons were trained to press a treadle in the presence of a discriminative compound stimulus either to avoid shock or to obtain a food reinforcer. The compound stimulus was composed of diffuse tone and light cues which had identical temporal patterns of onset, duration and offset. With the avoidance schedule the auditory cue acquired more control than the visual cue; however, when food was the reinforcer, the visual cue exerted more control. This pattern of stimulus control on the appetitive schedule did not change if random shocks were also added, even though these shocks were equal in density to the food presentations and equal in magnitude to those used for the avoidance schedule. Other changes in the appetitive procedure, such as making the tone spatially contiguous with food and removing the light in the food hopper, also failed to alter the relative control by the different cues. Prior training with a food reinforcer did not produce any change in the relative control by the two cues when the birds were retrained on the shock-avoidance schedule. The results suggest that some frequently stated alternatives to selective associability are not adequate explanations of this instance of a stimulus-reinforcer interaction. 相似文献
25.
Arne L. Ostergaard Michael Wilson Peter Clifton Michael Morgan Freda Newcombe J. Graham Beaumont Donald Broadbent 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1986,38(3):513-525
Arnold, M. Memory and the brain. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1984. Pp. 532. ISBN 0-89859-290-9. £47.90.
Green, T. R. G., Payne, S. P. and van der Veer, G. C. (Eds.). The psychology of computer use.. London: Academic Press. 1983. Pp. 225. ISBN 0-12-2974204. $9.50.
Stunkard, A. J. and Stellar, E. (Eds.). Eating and its disorders. New York: Raven Press. 1984. Pp. 302. ISBN 0-89004-891-6. $58.50.
Spillmann, L. and Wooten, B. R. (Eds.) Sensory experience, adaptation, and perception: Festschrift for Zero Kohler. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1984. Pp. xxvii + 748. ISBN 0 8985-3-218-6. £70.85.
Geschwind, N. and Galaburda, A. M. (Eds.)Cerebral dominance: The biological foundations. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. 1984. Pp. 232. ISBN 0-674-10658-X. £24.35.
Annett, M. Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. 1985. London and Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pp. xiii + 474. ISBN 0-86377418-5 £29.95.
Ericsson, K. A. and Simon, H. A. Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, Mass.: The M.I.T. Press. 1984. Pp. 426. ISBN 0-262-05029-3. £28.95. 相似文献
Green, T. R. G., Payne, S. P. and van der Veer, G. C. (Eds.). The psychology of computer use.. London: Academic Press. 1983. Pp. 225. ISBN 0-12-2974204. $9.50.
Stunkard, A. J. and Stellar, E. (Eds.). Eating and its disorders. New York: Raven Press. 1984. Pp. 302. ISBN 0-89004-891-6. $58.50.
Spillmann, L. and Wooten, B. R. (Eds.) Sensory experience, adaptation, and perception: Festschrift for Zero Kohler. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1984. Pp. xxvii + 748. ISBN 0 8985-3-218-6. £70.85.
Geschwind, N. and Galaburda, A. M. (Eds.)Cerebral dominance: The biological foundations. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. 1984. Pp. 232. ISBN 0-674-10658-X. £24.35.
Annett, M. Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. 1985. London and Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pp. xiii + 474. ISBN 0-86377418-5 £29.95.
Ericsson, K. A. and Simon, H. A. Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, Mass.: The M.I.T. Press. 1984. Pp. 426. ISBN 0-262-05029-3. £28.95. 相似文献
26.
27.
28.
Christopher D. Adams Anthony Dickinson 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1981,33(2):109-121
In two experiments, hungry rats were given instrumental lever-press training for an appetitive reinforcer and, in addition, were exposed to another type of food which was not contingent on lever pressing. In the first experiment, exposure to each type of food was on separate days, whereas in the second experiment rats were exposed to each type of food in strict alternation within each session. Subsequently, a food aversion was conditioned to the reinforcer for the experimental group and to the non-contingent food for the control group. In both experiments, animals with an aversion to the reinforcer responded less in an extinction test than animals with an aversion to the non-contingent food. Subsequent reacquisition tests confirmed that the aversion to the non-contingent food in the control group was of comparable strength with that to the reinforcer in the experimental group. The results were discussed in terms of whether the reinforcer is encoded in the associative structure set up by exposure to an instrumental contingency. 相似文献
29.
30.