全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5898篇 |
免费 | 114篇 |
国内免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
6024篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 606篇 |
2011年 | 716篇 |
2010年 | 165篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 517篇 |
2007年 | 521篇 |
2006年 | 509篇 |
2005年 | 408篇 |
2004年 | 365篇 |
2003年 | 403篇 |
2002年 | 352篇 |
2001年 | 250篇 |
2000年 | 270篇 |
1999年 | 136篇 |
1998年 | 28篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1959年 | 16篇 |
1958年 | 51篇 |
1957年 | 26篇 |
1956年 | 27篇 |
1955年 | 28篇 |
1954年 | 26篇 |
1953年 | 24篇 |
1952年 | 19篇 |
1951年 | 24篇 |
1950年 | 25篇 |
1949年 | 12篇 |
1948年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有6024条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
Sit-and-wait strategies in dynamic visual search 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The role of memory in visual search has lately become a controversial issue. Horowitz and Wolfe (1998) observed that performance in a visual search task was little affected by whether the stimuli were static or randomly relocated every 111 ms. Because a memory-based mechanism, such as inhibition of return, would be of no use in the dynamic condition, Horowitz and Wolfe concluded that memory is likewise not involved in the static condition. However, Horowitz and Wolfe could not effectively rule out the possibility that observers adopted a different strategy in the dynamic condition than in the static condition. That is, in the dynamic condition observers may have attended to a subregion of the display and waited for the target to appear there (sit-and-wait strategy). This hypothesis is supported by experimental data showing that performance in their dynamic condition does not differ from performance in another dynamic condition in which observers are forced to adopt a sit-and-wait strategy by being presented with a limited region of the display only. 相似文献
242.
Low-frequency irregular words are named more slowly and are more error prone than low-frequency regular words (the regularity
effect). Rastle and Coltheart (1999) reported that this irregularity cost is modulated by the serial position of the irregular
grapheme-phoneme correspondence, such that words with early irregularities exhibit a larger cost than words with late ones.
They argued that these data implicate rule-based serial processing, and they also reported a successful simulation with a
model that has a rule-based serial component—the DRC model of reading aloud (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler,
2001). However, Zorzi (2000) also simulated these data with a model that operates solely in parallel. Furthermore, Kwantes
and Mewhort (1999) simulated these data with a serial processing model that has no rules for converting orthography to phonology.
The human data reported by Rastle and Coltheart therefore neither require a serial processing account, nor successfully discriminate
among a number of computational models of reading aloud. New data are presented wherein an interaction between the effects
of regularity and serial position of irregularity is again reported for human readers. The DRC model simulated this interaction;
no other implemented computational model does so. The present results are thus consistent with rule-based serial processing
in reading aloud. 相似文献
243.
Thirty-six university students were tested in a plausibility judgment task using a self-paced listening paradigm under no-interference
and two-digit load conditions. Listening times were longer at syntactically more complex portions of syntactically more complex
sentences, and greater loads led to increased listening times. However, listening times at syntactically more complex positions
in syntactically more complex sentences did not increase more than listening times at comparable positions in syntactically
simple sentences under digit load conditions. The results indicate that a concurrent memory load does not reduce the availability
of working memory resources used for on-line syntactic processing and, thus, provide evidence that the working memory system
used for assigning syntactic structure is separate from that measured by standard working memory tasks. 相似文献
244.
Shytle RD Silver AA Sheehan KH Wilkinson BJ Newman M Sanberg PR Sheehan D 《Assessment》2003,10(3):273-287
To address the lack of a simple and standardized instrument to assess overall illness severity of Tourette's disorder (TD), the authors developed and tested a 15-item scale to measure a broad range of common symptoms including tics, inattention, hyperactivity, obsessions, compulsions, aggression, and emotional symptoms. Independent investigators used the 15-item Tourette's Disorder Scale (TODS) to assess 60 TD patients who were taking part in a double-blind placebo-controlled multicenter 8-week treatment study. Interrater reliability, internal consistency, convergent and discriminant validity, and sensitivity to change were examined. The TODS was associated with good interrater reliability, excellent internal consistency, and favorable levels of validity and sensitivity to change. Individual TODS items showed good convergent and discriminant validity against other measures. The TODS is a simple, efficient way for clinicians and parents to rate the severity of multiple symptoms commonly found in patients with Tourette's disorder. 相似文献
245.
Faust D 《Assessment》2003,10(4):428-441
There are a number of very helpful, but often underutilized, principles and procedures that can augment decision making in clinical and legal settings. Psychologists often restrict their range of decision-making strategies and options--at the cost of maximizing diagnostic and predictive accuracy--in part as the result of "ontological-epistemological one-worldedness" (O-E O-W). However, no philosophical, logical, or scientific necessity demands strict consistency between views regarding the nature of psychological phenomena and views about how to best assess or learn about those phenomena. Relaxing this unnecessary and largely psychologically-based O-E O-W may promote greater comfort with and utilization of the methods that are discussed in this article for increasing judgmental accuracy. 相似文献
246.
247.
248.
249.
There is growing dissatisfaction with a dichotomized "anger-in" versus "anger-out" view of anger coping. Three studies using student and community adultsamples revealed a broader understanding of the nature of anger coping styles and led to the development of the new Behavioral Anger Response Questionnaire (BARQ). The BARQ is empirically derived and factorially validated and has good psychometrics. Results suggest that dichotomizing anger responses as "in" versus "out" is too coarse and that a 6-factor model may be more appropriate. The 6 factors identified here are Direct Anger-Out, Assertion, Support-Seeking, Diffusion, Avoidance, and Rumination. Women reported use of a wider range of anger coping styles, especially more social support-seeking and more use of anger diffusion strategies than men. 相似文献
250.
Belgum D 《The journal of pastoral care & counseling : JPCC》2003,57(1):39-44
The author, acknowledging the reality of homeless persons in most communities, explores the meanings and dynamics of homelessness, and the need to recognize the variety of participants needing to be recognized in appreciating the complexity of this segment of society. He raises the issue of how pastoral caregivers become involved in providing authentic care to this sub-culture and offers examples from his own experiences as a volunteer chaplain in the Salvation Army Corps. 相似文献