全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40635篇 |
免费 | 774篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
41414篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 481篇 |
2018年 | 684篇 |
2017年 | 635篇 |
2016年 | 740篇 |
2015年 | 530篇 |
2014年 | 630篇 |
2013年 | 3843篇 |
2012年 | 1062篇 |
2011年 | 1122篇 |
2010年 | 710篇 |
2009年 | 678篇 |
2008年 | 1038篇 |
2007年 | 899篇 |
2006年 | 834篇 |
2005年 | 695篇 |
2004年 | 695篇 |
2003年 | 730篇 |
2002年 | 674篇 |
2001年 | 1237篇 |
2000年 | 1201篇 |
1999年 | 914篇 |
1998年 | 416篇 |
1996年 | 511篇 |
1995年 | 496篇 |
1994年 | 482篇 |
1993年 | 450篇 |
1992年 | 935篇 |
1991年 | 825篇 |
1990年 | 846篇 |
1989年 | 748篇 |
1988年 | 744篇 |
1987年 | 708篇 |
1986年 | 713篇 |
1985年 | 704篇 |
1984年 | 622篇 |
1983年 | 514篇 |
1982年 | 408篇 |
1981年 | 417篇 |
1979年 | 581篇 |
1978年 | 437篇 |
1975年 | 531篇 |
1974年 | 523篇 |
1973年 | 585篇 |
1972年 | 568篇 |
1971年 | 500篇 |
1970年 | 445篇 |
1969年 | 427篇 |
1968年 | 537篇 |
1967年 | 459篇 |
1966年 | 445篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
301.
302.
Literature evidence documenting the occurrence of relaxation-induced anxiety is reviewed, and several hypothesized mechanisms to explain the phenomenon are discussed. Possible avenues for circumventing the problem in therapy are offered. Finally, a theoretical model is presented wherein the phenomenon is viewed with a broader framework designed to explain the development and maintenance of the more generalized anxiety disorders. That framework emphasizes the emergence of fear of somatic anxiety cues and fear of loss of control from more fundamental interpersonal anxieties. 相似文献
303.
H E Blanchard G W McConkie D Zola G S Wolverton 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1984,10(1):75-89
College students read short texts from a cathode-ray tube as their eye movements were being monitored. During selected fixations, the text was briefly masked and then it reappeared with one word changed. Subjects often were unaware that the word had changed. Sometimes they reported seeing the first presented word, sometimes the second presented word, and sometimes both. When only one word was reported, two factors were found to determine which one it was: the length of time a word was present during the fixation and the predictability of a word in its context. The results suggested that visual information is utilized for reading at a crucial period during the fixation and that this crucial period can occur at different times on different fixations. The pattern of responses suggested that the first letter of a word is not utilized before other letters and that letters are not scanned from left to right during a fixation. 相似文献
304.
D Deutsch 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》1984,113(3):413-416
First, because tonal hierarchies exist in the music of long-established traditions, we may assume that they confer certain processing advantages. Such hierarchies capitalize on certain properties of the pitch memory system that do not require a tonal setting for demonstration. Furthermore, they facilitate the generation of hierarchical representations of sequential patterns of pitches, and such representations enable considerable parsimony of encoding. Second, the listener's establishment of a tonal hierarchy involves a process of key attribution. This process draws not only on the identities of the notes presented but also draws on their orderings. Certain sequential grouping factors are involved here that can also be shown to exist outside a tonal setting. We are therefore dealing with an elaborate bootstrapping operation, through which both a key and a sequential representation are arrived at by the listener. 相似文献
305.
Awareness of the response after feedback training for changes in heart rate and sudomotor laterality
L E Roberts R J Williams R G Marlin T Farrell D Imiolo 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》1984,113(2):225-255
What is the relation between the ability to control visceral responding on a biofeedback task and the ability to report behaviors actually contributing to this performance? Subjects received biofeedback training for unidentified visceral responses and then gave written reports about what they had done to control the feedback displays. Independent judges were given these reports and, on the basis of knowledge about activities known to contribute to visceral activity, were asked to determine the visceral responses for which the subjects had been trained. The reports of subjects who succeeded at bidirectional control of heart rate (Experiment 1) or sudomotor laterality (Experiment 2) showed awareness of behaviors related to feedback as assessed by this procedure, whereas the reports of subjects who failed at bidirectional control did not. Subsequent experiments indicated that these results did not depend on a learning strategy that might have been specific to the initial studies. These findings call into question the view that people are unaware of what they have done to produce the response after training on biofeedback tasks. Earlier studies reporting lack of awareness in biofeedback are discussed in light of factors that affect the measurement of biofeedback learning and response awareness. 相似文献
306.
R A Rosellini J P DeCola M Plonsky D A Warren A J Stilman 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1984,10(3):346-359
Learned helplessness theory predicts that animals exposed to inescapable shock acquire an expectancy of response-reinforcer independence, which proactively interferes with learning of response-reinforcer dependence. The theory also predicts that this expectancy can increase sensitivity to subsequent instances of response-reinforcer independence. These experiments test the latter prediction in a paradigm that minimizes the confounding effects of shock-induced activity deficits. Rats were trained to respond for food, then given either escapable, inescapable, or no shock. Subsequently, they received two sessions of response-contingent food followed by sessions of noncontingent food deliveries. During this phase, inescapably shocked animals decreased responding faster than did controls. Experiment 2 replicated this finding with a different schedule of food delivery and a procedure that more directly minimized the possibility that the outcome is due to either direct or indirect shock-induced activity changes. These results support the prediction that uncontrollable aversive events can increase an animal's sensitivity to noncontingent response-reinforcer relationships. 相似文献
307.
308.
309.
Detection versus discrimination of visual orientation 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
The role of focused attention in vision is examined. Recent theories of attention hypothesize that serial search by focal attention is required for discrimination between different combinations of features. Experiments are reported which show that the mixture of a few (less than five) horizontal and vertical line segments embedded in an aggregate of diagonal line segments can be rapidly counted (also called 'subitizing') by a parallel (preattentive) process, while the discrimination between horizontal and vertical orientation requires serial search by shifting focal attention to each line segment. Thus detecting and counting targets that differ in orientation can be done in parallel by a preattentive process, whereas knowing 'what' the orientation of a target is (horizontal or vertical, ie of a single conspicuous feature) requires a serial search by focal attention. 相似文献
310.
Studies were made of rapid error correction movements in eight subjects performing a visually guided tracking task involving flexion-extension movements about the elbow. Subjects were required to minimize reaction times in this two-choice task. Errors in initial movement direction occurred in about 3% of the trials. Error correction times (time from initiation to reversal of movement in incorrect direction) ranged from 30-150 ms. The first sing of correction of the error movement was a suppression of the electromyographic (EMG) activity in the muscle producing the error movement. This suppression started as early as 20-40 ms after the initiation of the error-related EMG activity and as much as 50 ms before any overt sign of limb movement. The correction of the error movement was also accompanied by an increase in the drive to the muscle which moved the arm in the correct direction. This increased activity always occurred after the initiation of the error movement. it is concluded that the first step in the error correction, suppression of drive to the muscle producing the error movement, cannot be based on information from the moving limb. It is thus suggested that this earliest response to the error movement is based on central monitoring of the commands for movement. 相似文献