全文获取类型
收费全文 | 41519篇 |
免费 | 1546篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
43076篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 444篇 |
2018年 | 646篇 |
2017年 | 606篇 |
2016年 | 692篇 |
2015年 | 508篇 |
2014年 | 602篇 |
2013年 | 3893篇 |
2012年 | 1053篇 |
2011年 | 1100篇 |
2010年 | 704篇 |
2009年 | 667篇 |
2008年 | 995篇 |
2007年 | 890篇 |
2006年 | 841篇 |
2005年 | 704篇 |
2004年 | 682篇 |
2003年 | 713篇 |
2002年 | 695篇 |
2001年 | 1327篇 |
2000年 | 1289篇 |
1999年 | 994篇 |
1996年 | 533篇 |
1995年 | 495篇 |
1994年 | 488篇 |
1993年 | 479篇 |
1992年 | 978篇 |
1991年 | 895篇 |
1990年 | 897篇 |
1989年 | 778篇 |
1988年 | 777篇 |
1987年 | 775篇 |
1986年 | 772篇 |
1985年 | 767篇 |
1984年 | 673篇 |
1983年 | 556篇 |
1982年 | 435篇 |
1981年 | 458篇 |
1979年 | 653篇 |
1978年 | 472篇 |
1976年 | 436篇 |
1975年 | 590篇 |
1974年 | 605篇 |
1973年 | 659篇 |
1972年 | 625篇 |
1971年 | 559篇 |
1970年 | 503篇 |
1969年 | 495篇 |
1968年 | 597篇 |
1967年 | 531篇 |
1966年 | 489篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
This article proposes that the social systems family therapists are concerned with may usefully be conceived of as interactive processes. It discusses limitations to the organismic view, in which social systems are seen as autonomous entities with boundaries, internal structures, and self-perpetuating behaviors, and argues, from Maturana and Varela, that social systems are qualitatively different from organisms. It develops from chaos theory an alternative view in which social systems are seen as complex nonlinear processes. It proposes that this view can free the therapist from the notions of family dysfunction and the systemic function of symptoms, and from reifications in which the family is seen as having covert rules, keeping secrets, neutralizing the therapist, and the like. This view leads to a redefinition of the therapist's place in the system and to the conclusion that family therapists can only influence systemic process by working with individuals. 相似文献
942.
A Final Comment on the Case of the Family Environment Scale 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
943.
When representations conflict with reality: the preschooler's problem with false beliefs and "false" photographs 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
D Zaitchik 《Cognition》1990,35(1):41-68
It has been argued that young preschoolers cannot correctly attribute a false belief to a deceived actor (Wimmer & Perner, 1983). Some researchers claim that the problem lies in the child's inadequate epistemology (Chandler & Boyes, 1982; Wellman, 1988); as such, it is specific to the child's theory of mind and no such problem should appear in reasoning about nonmental representations. This prediction is tested below in the "false photograph" task: here an actor takes a photograph of an object in location X; the object is then moved to location Y. Preschool subjects are asked: "In the picture, where is the object?" Results indicate that photographs are no easier to reason about than are beliefs. Manipulations to boost performance on the photograph task proved ineffective. Further, an explanation of the failure as a processing limitation having nothing to do with the representational nature of beliefs or photographs was ruled out. It is argued that young children's failure on the false belief task is not due to an inadequate epistemology (though they may have one) and is symptomatic of a larger problem with representations. 相似文献
944.
Screening batteries of standard neuropsychological tests were administered to 2 different samples (Ns = 90 and 167) of male prison inmates. Scores on the revised Psychopathy Checklist were used to divide inmates in each sample into high, moderate, and low psychopathy groups. There were no group differences in test performance in either of the samples, even when the effects of self-reported psychopathology and substance abuse were taken into account. The overall prevalence of both test-specific and global neuropsychological impairment was low and did not vary significantly across the 3 groups. The results provide no support for traditional brain-damage explanations of psychopathy. 相似文献
945.
D N Klein 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1990,99(4):412-421
The reliability and validity of Schneider's (1958) construct of depressive personality was evaluated in a sample of 177 outpatients, who were administered structured diagnostic and family history interviews, an extensive battery of inventories, and a 6-month follow-up assessment. The criteria for depressive personality had moderate to good interrater reliability, internal consistency, and test-retest stability, and the assessment of depressive personality traits was not influenced by patients' clinical states. In addition, preliminary support for the convergent and discriminant validity of the depressive personality construct was obtained. Although there were significant relations between the depressive personality and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. and rev. 3rd ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1987) diagnoses of dysthymia, the depressive personality was not entirely subsumed by existing mood disorders. 相似文献
946.
A case study is presented of the treatment of an Arab coed by a Jewish trainee, and a phantom cotherapist using a modified dialectical therapeutic approach. Its effect on the treatment process and outcome, and on the therapist is discussed. 相似文献
947.
Kathleen J. Zavela Jeffrey E. Barnett Keith John Smedi Joseph A. Istvan Joseph D. Matarazzo 《Journal of applied social psychology》1990,20(10):835-845
The interrelationships between cigarette, alcohol, and coffee use were examined in 307 U.S. army trainees and 304 U.S. enlisted army personnel. Usage patterns indicated distinct gender differences in this occupational setting. Among women, cigarette use and alcohol consumption were positively correlated; whereas for men, cigarette and coffee use were positively related. For both males and females, alcohol and coffee use were only weakly correlated. As demonstrated in other studies, nonsmokers showed a consistent pattern of abstention from coffee and alcohol or moderate alcohol use. 相似文献
948.
Two experiments evaluated the effect of commitment on paper recycling. Using an ABA design, Experiment 1 asked 24 individuals in a retirement home to sign a 4-week group commitment pledge to recycle paper. During this intervention, the subjects recycled 47% more paper than they had during baseline. They continued to recycle at this level during a 4-week follow-up period, when the group commitment was removed. Experiment 2 evaluated the relative effectiveness of group commitment, individual commitment, and token reinforcers on paper recycling in a college dormitory. The students in all three experimental conditions recycled from three to five times more paper than those in the control group during the 4-week intervention. However, in the 3-week follow-up period, when the treatments were removed, only the individually committed subjects continued to recycle significantly more paper than the controls. 相似文献
949.
A private key is a referential utterance used to inform selectively. The ambiguity of the utterance conceals meaning from nonselected audience members. The purpose of this study was to examine three aspects of children's understanding of the use of private keys. Experiment 1 concerned children's sensitivity to information in the context of utterance (i.e., the common ground). Experiment 2 examined appreciation of audience design and the speaker's intent. Experiments 3, 4, and 5 explored some reasons for children's problems in understanding private keys. The results show that even first graders are sensitive to common ground information and the speaker's intent. However, audience design is troublesome even for second graders in some situations, probably because they fail to assess the knowledge of the audience members. Private keys are important theoretically because they represent an aspect of referential communication involving the uses of ambiguous utterances that have been ignored by developmental researchers. 相似文献
950.
The present study investigates effects of different types of instructions (high-rate, interval, and minimal) during training with a fixed-interval schedule as a function of prior acquired temporal knowledge. A pretest was used to assess 4 1/2- and 6-year-old children's ability to understand the temporal parameters of a fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement. The results as a whole show that the control exerted by instructions given by the experimenter or elaborated by the subjects themselves on fixed-interval performance of young children depends on the interaction of two factors: development of verbal self-control skills and mastery of knowledge required by the rules forming the instructions. 相似文献