全文获取类型
收费全文 | 83260篇 |
免费 | 3354篇 |
国内免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
86643篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 926篇 |
2019年 | 1104篇 |
2018年 | 1568篇 |
2017年 | 1563篇 |
2016年 | 1670篇 |
2015年 | 1188篇 |
2014年 | 1429篇 |
2013年 | 7435篇 |
2012年 | 2623篇 |
2011年 | 2663篇 |
2010年 | 1628篇 |
2009年 | 1636篇 |
2008年 | 2353篇 |
2007年 | 2255篇 |
2006年 | 2068篇 |
2005年 | 1744篇 |
2004年 | 1779篇 |
2003年 | 1750篇 |
2002年 | 1652篇 |
2001年 | 2705篇 |
2000年 | 2548篇 |
1999年 | 1950篇 |
1998年 | 918篇 |
1997年 | 797篇 |
1996年 | 963篇 |
1995年 | 895篇 |
1994年 | 871篇 |
1993年 | 823篇 |
1992年 | 1740篇 |
1991年 | 1582篇 |
1990年 | 1587篇 |
1989年 | 1460篇 |
1988年 | 1448篇 |
1987年 | 1361篇 |
1986年 | 1329篇 |
1985年 | 1399篇 |
1984年 | 1181篇 |
1983年 | 1005篇 |
1979年 | 1193篇 |
1978年 | 863篇 |
1975年 | 1024篇 |
1974年 | 1092篇 |
1973年 | 1165篇 |
1972年 | 1029篇 |
1971年 | 935篇 |
1970年 | 808篇 |
1969年 | 791篇 |
1968年 | 1020篇 |
1967年 | 894篇 |
1966年 | 853篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
The major theories and research approaches as to the etiology and development of stuttering are reviewed from a historical perspective. Findings are drawn from different sources indicating that language deficits are an initial contributing factor and a continuing component of this disorder. Those subsets of children which constitute high-risk groups are identified. It is suggested that researchers begin investigating the underlying organic bases of stuttering and its relationship to stages of language development. Finally, clinicians are encouraged to employ a therapy program that is concerned with all areas of language, rather than emphasizing treating only the symptoms of stuttering behavior. 相似文献
992.
E Soetens M Deboeck J Hueting 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1984,10(4):581-598
A mathematical model is developed to describe sequential effects in two-choice reaction time experiments with a short response-stimulus interval. Evidence is briefly discussed that in conditions with short response-stimulus intervals, automatic aftereffects dominate sequential effects, and the influence of subjective expectancy can be neglected. In these conditions the model premises three components of automatic aftereffects--facilitation, inhibition, and noise, with a common decay factor. Influence of response-stimulus interval and practice on sequential effects are examined and related to parameter changes in the proposed single-decay model. The decrease of automatic aftereffects with increasing response-stimulus interval is primarily ascribed to an increasing decay factor. The parameter representation of the model also clarifies the issue of the disappearance of automatic aftereffects with practice. It shows a gradual fading of inhibition in the initial stages of practice, together with a slower decrease of the facilitation effect. The single-decay model provides a satisfactory explanation for the processes involved in compatible two-choice reaction time with short response-stimulus interval. 相似文献
993.
D Deutsch 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》1984,113(3):413-416
First, because tonal hierarchies exist in the music of long-established traditions, we may assume that they confer certain processing advantages. Such hierarchies capitalize on certain properties of the pitch memory system that do not require a tonal setting for demonstration. Furthermore, they facilitate the generation of hierarchical representations of sequential patterns of pitches, and such representations enable considerable parsimony of encoding. Second, the listener's establishment of a tonal hierarchy involves a process of key attribution. This process draws not only on the identities of the notes presented but also draws on their orderings. Certain sequential grouping factors are involved here that can also be shown to exist outside a tonal setting. We are therefore dealing with an elaborate bootstrapping operation, through which both a key and a sequential representation are arrived at by the listener. 相似文献
994.
Awareness of the response after feedback training for changes in heart rate and sudomotor laterality
L E Roberts R J Williams R G Marlin T Farrell D Imiolo 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》1984,113(2):225-255
What is the relation between the ability to control visceral responding on a biofeedback task and the ability to report behaviors actually contributing to this performance? Subjects received biofeedback training for unidentified visceral responses and then gave written reports about what they had done to control the feedback displays. Independent judges were given these reports and, on the basis of knowledge about activities known to contribute to visceral activity, were asked to determine the visceral responses for which the subjects had been trained. The reports of subjects who succeeded at bidirectional control of heart rate (Experiment 1) or sudomotor laterality (Experiment 2) showed awareness of behaviors related to feedback as assessed by this procedure, whereas the reports of subjects who failed at bidirectional control did not. Subsequent experiments indicated that these results did not depend on a learning strategy that might have been specific to the initial studies. These findings call into question the view that people are unaware of what they have done to produce the response after training on biofeedback tasks. Earlier studies reporting lack of awareness in biofeedback are discussed in light of factors that affect the measurement of biofeedback learning and response awareness. 相似文献
995.
R A Rosellini J P DeCola M Plonsky D A Warren A J Stilman 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1984,10(3):346-359
Learned helplessness theory predicts that animals exposed to inescapable shock acquire an expectancy of response-reinforcer independence, which proactively interferes with learning of response-reinforcer dependence. The theory also predicts that this expectancy can increase sensitivity to subsequent instances of response-reinforcer independence. These experiments test the latter prediction in a paradigm that minimizes the confounding effects of shock-induced activity deficits. Rats were trained to respond for food, then given either escapable, inescapable, or no shock. Subsequently, they received two sessions of response-contingent food followed by sessions of noncontingent food deliveries. During this phase, inescapably shocked animals decreased responding faster than did controls. Experiment 2 replicated this finding with a different schedule of food delivery and a procedure that more directly minimized the possibility that the outcome is due to either direct or indirect shock-induced activity changes. These results support the prediction that uncontrollable aversive events can increase an animal's sensitivity to noncontingent response-reinforcer relationships. 相似文献
996.
997.
998.
999.
1000.