首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   66406篇
  免费   2544篇
  国内免费   17篇
  2019年   779篇
  2018年   1098篇
  2017年   1073篇
  2016年   1160篇
  2015年   831篇
  2014年   1014篇
  2013年   5703篇
  2012年   1807篇
  2011年   1903篇
  2010年   1176篇
  2009年   1156篇
  2008年   1734篇
  2007年   1619篇
  2006年   1523篇
  2005年   1328篇
  2004年   1280篇
  2003年   1265篇
  2002年   1253篇
  2001年   2023篇
  2000年   1922篇
  1999年   1464篇
  1998年   751篇
  1996年   761篇
  1995年   728篇
  1994年   712篇
  1992年   1352篇
  1991年   1276篇
  1990年   1229篇
  1989年   1179篇
  1988年   1170篇
  1987年   1119篇
  1986年   1141篇
  1985年   1174篇
  1984年   1003篇
  1983年   881篇
  1982年   730篇
  1981年   731篇
  1979年   1029篇
  1978年   769篇
  1976年   695篇
  1975年   924篇
  1974年   963篇
  1973年   1025篇
  1972年   921篇
  1971年   819篇
  1970年   770篇
  1969年   778篇
  1968年   948篇
  1967年   831篇
  1966年   809篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
The country‐of‐origin effect is a topic central to the field of international marketing. Country of origin has been found to exert a particularly potent effect on consumer evaluation in situations where there is a strong link between a country and a particular product category. The present study provides further insight into how this particular effect can be understood. Drawing on a novel conceptualization of how country image and product categories interact, this study tested the relative evaluative relevance of product category with respect to estimates of brand equity across a variety of product categories. The findings suggest that facets of a country's image that are more closely related to the evaluation situation exert a greater influence on the evaluation of brands. This result encourages scholars as well as practitioners to re‐evaluate which situations might cause the country of origin effect to hold managerial relevance and paves the way for new paths toward a more comprehensive understanding of the effect. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
92.
Indiana statute allows police to seize firearms without a warrant if the officer believes a person meets the law's definition of “dangerous.” Review of the use of this law in Marion County (Indianapolis), Indiana, showed that prosecutors filed petitions in court to retain weapons seized by police under this law 404 times between 2006 and 2013. Police removed weapons from people due to identification of a risk of suicide (68%) or violence (21%), or the presence of psychosis (16%). The firearm seizures occurred in the context of domestic disputes in 28% of cases and intoxication was noted in 26% of cases. There were significant demographic differences in the circumstances of firearm seizures and the firearms seized. The seized firearms were retained by the court at the initial hearing in 63% of cases; this retention was closely linked to the defendant's failure to appear at the hearing. The court dismissed 29% of cases at the initial hearing, closely linked to the defendant's presence at the hearing. In subsequent hearings of cases not dismissed, the court ordered the destruction of the firearms in 72% of cases, all when the individual did not appear in court, and dismissed 24% of the cases, all when the individual was present at the hearing. Overall, the Indiana law removed weapons from a small number of people, most of whom did not seek return of their weapons. The firearm seizure law thus functioned as a months-long cooling-off period for those who did seek the return of their guns. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
93.
94.
95.
96.
97.
98.
99.
In Experiment 1, rats were trained on a discrimination between rubber- and sandpaper-covered arms of a maze after one group had been pre-exposed to these intra-maze cues. Pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made easier by adding further discriminative stimuli, when it now significantly retarded learning. In Experiment 2, rats were trained on an extra-maze spatial discrimination, again after one group, but not another, had been pre-exposed to the extra-maze landmarks. Here too, pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made substantially easier by arranging that the two arms between which rats had to choose were always separated by 135°. The results of both experiments can be explained by supposing that perceptual learning depends on the presence of features common to S+ and S-.  相似文献   
100.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号