首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   66406篇
  免费   2544篇
  国内免费   17篇
  2019年   779篇
  2018年   1098篇
  2017年   1073篇
  2016年   1160篇
  2015年   831篇
  2014年   1014篇
  2013年   5703篇
  2012年   1807篇
  2011年   1903篇
  2010年   1176篇
  2009年   1156篇
  2008年   1734篇
  2007年   1619篇
  2006年   1523篇
  2005年   1328篇
  2004年   1280篇
  2003年   1265篇
  2002年   1253篇
  2001年   2023篇
  2000年   1922篇
  1999年   1464篇
  1998年   751篇
  1996年   761篇
  1995年   728篇
  1994年   712篇
  1992年   1352篇
  1991年   1276篇
  1990年   1229篇
  1989年   1179篇
  1988年   1170篇
  1987年   1119篇
  1986年   1141篇
  1985年   1174篇
  1984年   1003篇
  1983年   881篇
  1982年   730篇
  1981年   731篇
  1979年   1029篇
  1978年   769篇
  1976年   695篇
  1975年   924篇
  1974年   963篇
  1973年   1025篇
  1972年   921篇
  1971年   819篇
  1970年   770篇
  1969年   778篇
  1968年   948篇
  1967年   831篇
  1966年   809篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号