首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   55868篇
  免费   2120篇
  国内免费   21篇
  2019年   610篇
  2018年   888篇
  2017年   884篇
  2016年   986篇
  2015年   723篇
  2014年   851篇
  2013年   5066篇
  2012年   1442篇
  2011年   1512篇
  2010年   955篇
  2009年   909篇
  2008年   1318篇
  2007年   1283篇
  2006年   1150篇
  2005年   1031篇
  2004年   972篇
  2003年   999篇
  2002年   999篇
  2001年   1782篇
  2000年   1701篇
  1999年   1316篇
  1998年   571篇
  1996年   674篇
  1995年   643篇
  1994年   603篇
  1993年   583篇
  1992年   1259篇
  1991年   1169篇
  1990年   1151篇
  1989年   1040篇
  1988年   1060篇
  1987年   1003篇
  1986年   1030篇
  1985年   1010篇
  1984年   871篇
  1983年   728篇
  1982年   584篇
  1981年   602篇
  1979年   874篇
  1978年   663篇
  1975年   776篇
  1974年   825篇
  1973年   908篇
  1972年   809篇
  1971年   741篇
  1970年   692篇
  1969年   648篇
  1968年   840篇
  1967年   716篇
  1966年   679篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号