全文获取类型
收费全文 | 63319篇 |
免费 | 2375篇 |
国内免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
65723篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 741篇 |
2018年 | 1052篇 |
2017年 | 1054篇 |
2016年 | 1099篇 |
2015年 | 787篇 |
2014年 | 946篇 |
2013年 | 5558篇 |
2012年 | 1639篇 |
2011年 | 1759篇 |
2010年 | 1153篇 |
2009年 | 1120篇 |
2008年 | 1642篇 |
2007年 | 1497篇 |
2006年 | 1398篇 |
2005年 | 1183篇 |
2004年 | 1107篇 |
2003年 | 1201篇 |
2002年 | 1101篇 |
2001年 | 2119篇 |
2000年 | 2071篇 |
1999年 | 1541篇 |
1998年 | 701篇 |
1996年 | 712篇 |
1995年 | 688篇 |
1994年 | 697篇 |
1993年 | 655篇 |
1992年 | 1437篇 |
1991年 | 1340篇 |
1990年 | 1329篇 |
1989年 | 1195篇 |
1988年 | 1161篇 |
1987年 | 1165篇 |
1986年 | 1126篇 |
1985年 | 1186篇 |
1984年 | 996篇 |
1983年 | 805篇 |
1981年 | 658篇 |
1979年 | 957篇 |
1978年 | 719篇 |
1976年 | 648篇 |
1975年 | 886篇 |
1974年 | 876篇 |
1973年 | 1001篇 |
1972年 | 868篇 |
1971年 | 760篇 |
1970年 | 703篇 |
1969年 | 689篇 |
1968年 | 854篇 |
1967年 | 741篇 |
1966年 | 775篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
23.
D. THOMAS PORTER 《人类交流研究》1981,8(1):58-71
Since 1970 about 80% of the research on oral communication apprehension has used one of two versions of McCroskey's Personal Report of Communication Apprehension. The original 20-item PRCA was introduced for the purpose of measuring apprehension beyond the traditional public speaking context. Although the PRCA-20 is reliable, some serious criticism of it has centered on its ability to tap interpersonal behavior outcomes. In an attempt to bolster its interpersonal applicability, five “interpersonally-oriented” items were added to form the PRCA-25. The studies reported herein demonstrate that both versions fail to possess sufficient reliable variance to be applicable to contexts other than public settings. In addition, it was found that the interpersonal items are a separate component of the PRCA, and as such, do not justify being treated equal in terms of additivity and, therefore, reduce the construct integrity of the PRCA. 相似文献
24.
25.
Christopher D. Adams Anthony Dickinson 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1981,33(2):109-121
In two experiments, hungry rats were given instrumental lever-press training for an appetitive reinforcer and, in addition, were exposed to another type of food which was not contingent on lever pressing. In the first experiment, exposure to each type of food was on separate days, whereas in the second experiment rats were exposed to each type of food in strict alternation within each session. Subsequently, a food aversion was conditioned to the reinforcer for the experimental group and to the non-contingent food for the control group. In both experiments, animals with an aversion to the reinforcer responded less in an extinction test than animals with an aversion to the non-contingent food. Subsequent reacquisition tests confirmed that the aversion to the non-contingent food in the control group was of comparable strength with that to the reinforcer in the experimental group. The results were discussed in terms of whether the reinforcer is encoded in the associative structure set up by exposure to an instrumental contingency. 相似文献
26.
Although perceived health risk plays a prominent role in theories of health behavior, its empirical role in risk taking is less clear. In Study 1 (N = 129), 2 measures of drivers' risk-taking behavior were found to be unrelated to self-estimates of accident concern but to be related to self-ratings of driving skill and the perceived thrill of driving. In Study 2 (N = 405), out of a wide range of potential influences, accident concern had the weakest relationship with risk taking. The authors concluded that although health risk is a key feature in many theories of health behavior and a central focus for researchers and policy makers, it may not be such a prominent factor for those actually taking the risk. 相似文献
27.
28.
29.
30.