全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1201篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1219篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 36篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 108篇 |
2012年 | 55篇 |
2011年 | 61篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 64篇 |
2007年 | 61篇 |
2006年 | 44篇 |
2005年 | 52篇 |
2004年 | 64篇 |
2003年 | 35篇 |
2002年 | 49篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1219条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
Anne Shaffer Cynthia Suveg Kristel Thomassin Laura L. Bradbury 《Journal of child and family studies》2012,21(6):917-924
The current study expands upon the extant literature by examining the influence of contextual risk factors (e.g., parental psychological distress, household income, educational attainment) on parental responses to children’s emotional experiences. Participants included 97 ethnically and demographically diverse mother–child dyads with children ranging in age from 7 to 12. Mothers reported indicators of supportive and unsupportive emotion socialization practices, and measures of child emotion regulation and emotion dysregulation. Higher scores on the familial risk index were positively related to increased emotion dysregulation and negatively related to decreased emotion regulation through mediated effects of mothers’ unsupportive reactions to children’s negative emotional expressions. These findings suggest the importance of considering contextual influences on the emotion socialization process and offer potential avenues to foster adaptive emotional development in the context of high risk. 相似文献
822.
To reach a greater understanding of the early father-child attachment relationship, this study examined concurrent and longitudinal associations among father involvement, paternal sensitivity, and father-child attachment security at 13 months and 3 years of age. Analyses revealed few associations among these variables at 13 months of age, but involvement and sensitivity independently predicted father-child attachment security at age 3. Moreover, sensitivity moderated the association between involvement and attachment security at 3 years. Specifically, involvement was unrelated to attachment security when fathers were highly sensitive, but positively related to attachment security when fathers were relatively less sensitive. Father involvement was also moderately stable across the two time points, but paternal sensitivity was not. Furthermore, there was significant stability in father-child attachment security from 13 months to 3 years. Secure attachment at 13 months also predicted greater levels of paternal sensitivity at 3 years, with sensitivity at age 3 mediating the association between 13 month and 3 year attachment security. In sum, a secure father-child attachment relationship (a) was related to both quantity and quality of fathering behavior, (b) remained relatively stable across early childhood, and (c) predicted increased paternal sensitivity over time. These findings further our understanding of the correlates of early father-child attachment, and underscore the need to consider multiple domains of fathers' parenting and reciprocal relations between fathering behavior and father-child attachment security. 相似文献
823.
People consistently believe that negative events are less likely to happen to them than to others. Research suggests a relationship between this optimistic bias and perceived control such that the greater control people perceive over future events, the greater their optimistic bias. We conducted a meta-analysis of 27 independent samples to quantify the size of this relationship and examine what variables moderated the relationship. Greater perceived control was significantly related to greater optimistic bias, but this relationship was moderated by participant nationality, student status, risk status, and the type of optimistic bias measure used. We discuss the findings in the context of primary versus secondary control. 相似文献
824.
Cynthia S. Cycyota Claudia J. Ferrante Steven G. Green Kurt A. Heppard Dorri M. Karolick 《Journal of Academic Ethics》2011,9(3):177-192
We describe the educational character and leadership development processes used by the United States Air Force Academy that
other educational institutions may find useful. Our processes include an integrated educational curriculum designed to complement
and integrate the experiential learning that results in achieving specific organizational outcomes, co-curricular activities
in cadet living, and a specific focus on the ethical development of leaders’ respect for human dignity and cultural competency
as well as the mechanisms to assess and refine our processes. 相似文献
825.
826.
Jianghong Liu Halina Cheng Patrick W. L. Leung 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2011,39(2):251-264
Preschool children have long been a neglected population in the study of psychopathology. The Achenbach System of Empirically
Based Assessment (ASEBA), which includes the Child Behavior Checklist/1.5-5 (CBCL/1.5-5) and the Caregiver-Teacher Report
Form (C-TRF), constitutes the few available measures to assess preschoolers with an empirically derived taxonomy of preschool
psychopathology. However, the utility of the measures and their taxonomy of preschool psychopathology to the Chinese is largely
unknown and has not been studied. The present study aimed at testing the cross-cultural factorial validity of the CBCL/1.5-5
and C-TRF, as well as the applicability of the taxonomy of preschool psychopathology they embody, to Mainland Chinese preschoolers.
Country effects between our Chinese sample and the original U.S. sample, gender differences, and cross-informant agreement
between teachers and parents were also to be examined. A Chinese version of the CBCL/1.5-5 and C-TRF was completed by parents
and teachers respectively on 876 preschoolers in Mainland China. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) confirmed the original,
U.S.-derived second order, multi-factor model best fit the Chinese preschool data of the CBCL/1.5-5 and C-TRF. Rates of total
behavior problems in Chinese preschoolers were largely similar to those in American preschoolers. Specifically, Chinese preschoolers
scored higher on internalizing problems while American preschoolers scored higher on externalizing problems. Chinese preschool
boys had significantly higher rates of externalizing problems than Chinese preschool girls. Cross-informant agreement between
Chinese teachers and parents was relatively low compared to agreement in the original U.S. sample. Results support the generalizability
of the taxonomic structure of preschool psychopathology derived in the U.S. to the Chinese, as well as the applicability of
the Chinese version of the CBCL/1.5-5 and C-TRF. 相似文献
827.
Perceptions of cognitive compensation and interpersonal enjoyment of collaboration were examined in three hundred middle-aged and older couples who completed measures of perceptions of collaboration, cognitive ability, marital satisfaction, an errand task and judged their spouse's affiliation. Older adults (especially men) endorsed cognitive compensation and interpersonal enjoyment and reported using collaboration more frequently than middle-aged adults. Greater need for cognitive compensation was related to lower cognitive ability only for older wives. Greater marital satisfaction was associated with greater interpersonal enjoyment. These two functions related to reports of more frequent use of collaboration and perceptions of spousal affiliation in a collaborative task. 相似文献
828.
Stanton SJ Mullette-Gillman OA McLaurin RE Kuhn CM LaBar KS Platt ML Huettel SA 《Psychological science》2011,22(4):447-453
Testosterone is positively associated with risk-taking behavior in social domains (e.g., crime, physical aggression). However, the scant research linking testosterone to economic risk preferences presents inconsistent findings. We examined the relationship between endogenous testosterone and individuals' economic preferences (i.e., risk preference, ambiguity preference, and loss aversion) in a large sample (N = 298) of men and women. We found that endogenous testosterone levels have a significant U-shaped association with individuals' risk and ambiguity preferences, but not loss aversion. Specifically, individuals with low or high levels of testosterone (more than 1.5 SD from the mean for their gender) were risk and ambiguity neutral, whereas individuals with intermediate levels of testosterone were risk and ambiguity averse. This relationship was highly similar in men and women. In contrast to received wisdom regarding testosterone and risk, the present data provide the first robust evidence for a nonlinear association between economic preferences and levels of endogenous testosterone. 相似文献
829.
Helms, Henze, Sass, and Mifsud (2006) defined good practices for internal consistency reporting, interpretation, and analysis consistent with an alpha-as-data perspective. Their viewpoint (a) expands on previous arguments that reliability coefficients are group-level summary statistics of samples' responses rather than stable properties of scales or measures and (b) encourages researchers to investigate characteristics of reliability data for their own samples and subgroups within their samples. In Study 1, we reviewed past and current reliability reporting practices in a sample of Psychological Assessment articles published across 3 decades (i.e., from the years 1989, 1996, and 2006). Results suggested that contemporary and past researchers' reliability reporting practices have not improved over time and generally were not consistent with good practices. In Study 2, we analyzed an archival data set to illustrate the real-life repercussions of researchers' ongoing misconstrual and misuse of reliability data. Our analyses suggested that researchers should conduct preliminary analyses of their data to determine whether their data fit the assumptions of their reliability analyses. Also, the results indicated that reliability coefficients varied across racial or ethnic and gender subgroups, and these variations had implications for whether the same depression measure should be used across groups. We concluded that the alpha-as-data perspective has implications for one's choice of psychological measures and interpretation of results, which subsequently affect conclusions and recommendations. We encourage researchers to recognize the people behind their data by adopting better practices in internal consistency reporting, analysis, and interpretation. 相似文献
830.