全文获取类型
收费全文 | 318篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
332篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 34篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有332条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
72.
73.
74.
Groups of 30 ADD-H boys and 90 normal boys were divided into 30 mixed dyads composed of a normal and an ADD-H boy, and 30 normal dyads composed of 2 normal boys. Dyads were videotaped interacting in 15-minute free-play, 15-minute cooperative task, and 15-minute simulated classroom settings. Mixed dyads engaged in more controlling interaction than normal dyads in both free-play and simulated classroom settings. In the simulated classroom, mixed dyads completed fewer math problems and were less compliant with the commands of peers. ADD-H children spent less simulated classroom time on task and scored lower on drawing tasks than normal peers. Older dyads proved less controlling, more compliant with peer commands, more inclined to play and work independently, less active, and more likely to remain on task during the cooperative task and simulated classroom settings. Results suggest that the ADD-H child prompts a more controlling, less cooperative pattern of responses from normal peers. 相似文献
75.
We reply to the commentary in which Muter (1995) disputes the findings and arguments of Cunningham, Healy, Till, Fendrich, and Dimitry (1993) concerning the rate of forgetting from primary memory. Although Muter (1980) had reported very rapid forgetting when secondary memory processes were minimized, Cunningham et al. found that the rate of forgetting from primary memory was no more rapid when secondary memory processes were minimal than it was when secondary memory processes were substantial. We locate the discrepancy between the two studies specifically at recall performance on the 0-sec retention interval, and we argue that the very rapid forgetting found by Muter (1980) can be attributed in large part to the fact that although subjects in his experiments did not expect to recall the letters after a filled retention interval, at the time of encoding and after an unfilled delay they did have a high overall expectancy to recall the letters. 相似文献
76.
In the present study, we explored adaptation to prism-displaced dynamic and static events under conditions of minimal information. Many of our interactions with the world are dynamic and involve reaching for or intercepting moving objects. The consequences (or feedback) of those interactions entail the presence or absence of physical contact with the moving objects. In this study, humans learned, with only heptic feedback, to intercept optically displaced falling balls. To eliminate visual feedback, the falling balls disappeared behind an occluder (which systematically varied in size across groups) prior to either striking or missing a subject's hand. As occluder size decreased, adaptation increased. With minimum occluder sizes, the greatest adaptation occurred around the training position, and adaptation decreased as distance between training and testing positions increased. The results can best be described in terms of a generalization gradient centered around the training position. This generalization gradient was not present when subjects were trained with ecologically similar static arrays. Implications for models of adaptation are discussed. 相似文献
77.
78.
Cunningham PB Henggeler SW Limber SP Melton GB Nation MA 《Journal of clinical child psychology》2000,29(3):432-442
Examined prevalence of gun ownership and the links among gun ownership, reasons for gun ownership, and antisocial behavior in a sample of nonmetropolitan and rural middle school students. Participants completed the Questionnaire for Students (Olweus, 1995) and included 6,263 students from 36 elementary and middle schools, of whom most were African American (range = 46%-95% per school). Reasons for gun ownership were strongly associated with rates of antisocial behavior. Youths who owned guns for sporting reasons reported rates of antisocial behavior that were only slightly higher than those reported by youths who did not own guns. Youths who owned guns to gain respect or to frighten others reported extremely high rates of antisocial behavior. These high-risk adolescent gun owners were likely to come from families of high-risk gun owners, associate with friends who were high-risk gun owners, and engage in high rates of bullying behavior. Findings suggest that effective violence prevention programs must target high-risk youths, address risk factors that go beyond individual settings, and address a comprehensive array of risk factors. 相似文献
79.
Two studies investigated relationships among individual differences in implicit and explicit prejudice, right-wing ideology, and rigidity in thinking. The first study examined these relationships focusing on White Americans' prejudice toward Black Americans. The second study provided the first test of implicit ethnocentrism and its relationship to explicit ethnocentrism by studying the relationship between attitudes toward five social groups. Factor analyses found support for both implicit and explicit ethnocentrism. In both studies, mean explicit attitudes toward out groups were positive, whereas implicit attitudes were negative, suggesting that implicit and explicit prejudices are distinct; however, in both studies, implicit and explicit attitudes were related (r = .37, .47). Latent variable modeling indicates a simple structure within this ethnocentric system, with variables organized in order of specificity. These results lead to the conclusion that (a) implicit ethnocentrism exists and (b) it is related to and distinct from explicit ethnocentrism. 相似文献
80.
Jan?Peter?de?RuiterEmail author Stéphane?Rossignol Louis?Vuurpijl Douglas?W.?Cunningham Willem?J.?M.?Levelt 《Behavior research methods》2003,35(3):408-419
In this article, we present the spatial logistics task (SLOT) platform for investigating multimodal communication between
2 human participants. Presented are the SLOT communication task and the software and hardware that has been developed to run
SLOT experiments and record the participants’ multimodal behavior. SLOT offers a high level of flexibility in varying the
context of the communication and is particularly useful in studies of the relationship between pen gestures and speech. We
illustrate the use of the SLOT platform by discussing the results of some early experiments. The first is an experiment on
negotiation with a one-way mirror between the participants, and the second is an exploratory study of automatic recognition
of spontaneous pen gestures. The results of these studies demonstrate the usefulness of the SLOT platform for conducting multimodal
communication research in both human-human and human-computer interactions. 相似文献