首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1284篇
  免费   85篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   15篇
  2020年   28篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   32篇
  2017年   26篇
  2016年   36篇
  2015年   30篇
  2014年   25篇
  2013年   168篇
  2012年   58篇
  2011年   64篇
  2010年   42篇
  2009年   41篇
  2008年   56篇
  2007年   47篇
  2006年   49篇
  2005年   46篇
  2004年   53篇
  2003年   44篇
  2002年   42篇
  2001年   25篇
  2000年   22篇
  1999年   25篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   22篇
  1996年   26篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   19篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   10篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   9篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   8篇
  1968年   6篇
排序方式: 共有1370条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
123.
124.
125.
126.
Confidence intervals (CIs) give information about replication, but many researchers have misconceptions about this information. One problem is that the percentage of future replication means captured by a particular CI varies markedly, depending on where in relation to the population mean that CI falls. The authors investigated the distribution of this percentage for varsigma known and unknown, for various sample sizes, and for robust CIs. The distribution has strong negative skew: Most 95% CIs will capture around 90% or more of replication means, but some will capture a much lower proportion. On average, a 95% CI will include just 83.4% of future replication means. The authors present figures designed to assist understanding of what CIs say about replication, and they also extend the discussion to explain how p values give information about replication.  相似文献   
127.
In 3 studies, participants viewed sequences of multiattribute objects (e.g., colored shapes) appearing with varying frequencies and judged the likelihood of the attributes of those objects. Judged probabilities reflected a compromise between (a) the frequency with which each attribute appeared and (b) the ignorance prior probability cued by the number of distinct values that the focal attribute could take on. Thus, judged probabilities were partition dependent, varying with the number of events into which the state space was subjectively divided. This bias was diminished among participants more confident in what they learned, was strong and insensitive to level of confidence when ignorance priors were especially salient, and required ignorance priors to be salient only when probabilities were elicited (not during encoding).  相似文献   
128.
Previous studies have shown that presession attention for problem behavior can serve as an abolishing operation when attention functions as a positive reinforcer. In the current study, we show that the stereotypy of a child with severe disabilities was undifferentiated during standard analogue functional analysis conditions. However, when noncontingent presession attention was provided, stereotypy occurred for social attention as a positive reinforcer, suggesting that the antecedent manipulation functioned as an establishing operation.  相似文献   
129.
In each of two experiments, the comparative instructions in a symbolic comparison task were either varied randomly from trial to trial (mixed blocks) or left constant (pure blocks) within blocks of trials. In the first experiment, every stimulus was compared with every other stimulus. The symbolic distance effect (DE) was enhanced, and the semantic congruity effect (SCE) was significantly larger, when the instructions were randomized than when they were blocked. In a second experiment, each stimulus was paired with only one other stimulus. The SCE was again larger when instructions were randomized than when they were blocked. The enhanced SCE and DE with randomized instructions follow naturally from evidence accrual views of comparative judgments.  相似文献   
130.
The current research investigated applicant reactions derived from common application blanks. Previous work has found applications to be riddled with legally inadvisable items, but limited research has attempted to understand how such items might affect applicants. By using organizational justice theory as a framework, two studies examined applicant reactions from two application blanks: a legally advisable application and a legally problematic application. It was found that applicants completing the legally problematic application had lower perceptions of justice and expressed higher litigation intentions compared to those completing the legally advisable application, especially for those that were rejected without an explanation.
J. Craig WallaceEmail:
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号