全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1322篇 |
免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
1344篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 27篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 165篇 |
2012年 | 58篇 |
2011年 | 61篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 39篇 |
2008年 | 52篇 |
2007年 | 49篇 |
2006年 | 47篇 |
2005年 | 42篇 |
2004年 | 49篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1344条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
DuHamel KN Ostrof J Ashman T Winkel G Mundy EA Keane TM Morasco BJ Vickberg SM Hurley K Burkhalter J Chhabra R Scigliano E Papadopoulos E Moskowitz C Redd W 《心理评价》2004,16(3):255-266
The measurement of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is critically important for the identification and treatment of this disorder. The PTSD Checklist (PCL; F. W. Weathers and J. Ford, 1996) is a self-report measure that is increasingly used. In this study, the authors investigated the factorial validity of the PCL with data from 236 cancer survivors who received a bone marrow or stem cell transplantation. The authors examined the fit of these data with the clinical model of 3 symptom clusters for PTSD, as proposed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, and alternative models tested in prior research. By using confirmatory factor analysis the authors found that a 4-first-order-factor model of PTSD provided the best fit. The relations of PTSD symptoms with sociodemographic and medical variables were also explored. 相似文献
902.
The authors investigated the effects of attention to novelty, fear-anxiety, and age on 3 measures of conduct problems. They found several main and interaction effects. The results indicated the presence of the hypothesized 3-way interaction for 2 dependent measures (i.e., conduct problem, socialized aggression); the 3rd dependent measure (i.e., conduct disorder) approached significance (p = .07). Participants who were older, had low attention to novelty, and high fear-anxiety generally had the highest conduct-problem scores among 8 comparison groups. Older students with high attention to novelty and low fear-anxiety had significantly lower conduct-problem scores that were unremarkable. The findings are congruent with J. Gray's (1987) theory of behavioral activation and inhibition, and the results support R. C. Eaves' (1993) integrated theory of human behavior, which postulates that the level of attention to novelty, fear-anxiety, and age interact to produce individuals with conduct problems. 相似文献
903.
A. J. Ploszay Noah B. Gentner Christopher H. Skinner Craig A. Wrisberg 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2006,15(4):247-255
A multiple-baseline design was used to evaluate the effects of a pre-shot putting routine on the putting performance of four NCAA Division I golfers. The routine involved a combination of multisensory imagery and simulated putting movements. Results suggested that the intervention was effective for some participants. Discussion focuses on directions for future research and the use of single-subject design procedures in sport psychology and related disciplines.
相似文献
Craig A. WrisbergEmail: |
904.
905.
906.
McKenzie CR 《Memory & cognition》2006,34(3):577-588
Researchers have recently pointed out that neither biased testing nor biased evaluation of hypotheses necessitates confirmation bias--defined here as systematic overconfidence in a focal hypothesis--but certain testing/evaluation combinations do. One such combination is (1) a tendency to ask about features that are either very likely or very unlikely under the focal hypothesis (extremity bias) and (2) a tendency to treat confirming and disconfirming answers as more similar in terms of their diagnosticity (or informativeness) than they really are. However, in previous research showing the second tendency, materials that are highly abstract and unfamiliar have been used. Two experiments demonstrated that using familiar materials led participants to distinguish much better between the differential diagnosticity of confirming and disconfirming answers. The conditions under which confirmation bias is a serious concern might be quite limited. 相似文献
907.
Self-report and observational measures of pain are examined from the perspective of a model of human communication. This model examines the experience of pain as affected by intrapersonal and contextual factors, the process whereby it is encoded into expressive behaviour, and the process of decoding by observers prior to their engaging in action. Self-report measures primarily capture expressive pain behaviour that is under the control of higher mental processes, whereas observational measures capture behaviour that is less subject to voluntary control and more automatic. Automatic expressive behaviours are subject to less purposeful distortion than are behaviours dependent upon higher mental processes. Consequently, observational measures can be used and have clinical utility as indices of pain when self-report is not available, for example, in infants, young children, people with intellectual disabilities or brain damage, and seniors with dementia. These measures are also useful when the credibility of self-report is questioned and even when credible self-report is available. However, automatic behaviours may be more difficult for observers to decode. The model outlined herein takes into account the role of various human developmental stages in pain experience and expression and in understanding the utility of self-report and observational measures. We conclude that both observational and self-report measures are essential in the assessment of pain because of the unique information that each type contributes. 相似文献
908.
The clinical assessment of psychopathy in adulthood is well established via programmatic research. More recently, psychopathy has been extended to children and adolescents with correlates to maladaptive personality traits, violent behavior, and noncompliance with institutional rules. To screen for adolescent psychopathy, the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) was developed as a 20-item self-report measure of psychopathy. The original validation of the APSD was limited to samples of clinic-referred and community-based children. In extending this research to delinquent populations, the current article uses two separate samples of adolescent offenders incarcerated in a maximum security facility (n = 78) and a local juvenile detention facility (n = 77). As evidence of criterion-related validity, the APSD was compared with two versions of the Psychopathy Checklist that yielded mixed results. Construct validity was examined via a confirmatory factor analysis that provided support for a three-factor model of the APSD. 相似文献
909.
Framing effects are well established: Listeners’ preferences depend on how outcomes are described to them, or framed. Less
well understood is what determines how speakers choose frames. Two experiments revealed that reference points systematically
influenced speakers’ choices between logically equivalent frames. For example, speakers tended to describe a 4-ounce cup filled
to the 2-ounce line as half full if it was previously empty but described it as half empty if it was previously full. Similar
results were found when speakers could describe the outcome of a medical treatment in terms of either mortality or survival
(e.g., 25% die vs. 75% survive). Two additional experiments showed that listeners made accurate inferences about speakers’
reference points on the basis of the selected frame (e.g., if a speaker described a cup as half empty, listeners inferred
that the cup used to be full). Taken together, the data suggest that frames reliably convey implicit information in addition
to their explicit content, which helps explain why framing effects are so robust. 相似文献
910.