全文获取类型
收费全文 | 32443篇 |
免费 | 279篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
32723篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 106篇 |
2020年 | 166篇 |
2019年 | 178篇 |
2018年 | 3687篇 |
2017年 | 2997篇 |
2016年 | 2460篇 |
2015年 | 392篇 |
2014年 | 291篇 |
2013年 | 1020篇 |
2012年 | 930篇 |
2011年 | 2762篇 |
2010年 | 2734篇 |
2009年 | 1673篇 |
2008年 | 2015篇 |
2007年 | 2442篇 |
2006年 | 321篇 |
2005年 | 457篇 |
2004年 | 407篇 |
2003年 | 331篇 |
2002年 | 291篇 |
2001年 | 466篇 |
2000年 | 459篇 |
1999年 | 305篇 |
1998年 | 151篇 |
1997年 | 117篇 |
1996年 | 125篇 |
1995年 | 106篇 |
1993年 | 104篇 |
1992年 | 228篇 |
1991年 | 218篇 |
1990年 | 221篇 |
1989年 | 212篇 |
1988年 | 214篇 |
1987年 | 193篇 |
1986年 | 180篇 |
1985年 | 186篇 |
1984年 | 173篇 |
1983年 | 126篇 |
1982年 | 125篇 |
1979年 | 159篇 |
1975年 | 143篇 |
1974年 | 165篇 |
1973年 | 187篇 |
1972年 | 159篇 |
1971年 | 143篇 |
1970年 | 127篇 |
1969年 | 136篇 |
1968年 | 181篇 |
1967年 | 135篇 |
1966年 | 144篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
801.
Eunjeong?KangEmail author Ana María?Brannan Craig Anne?Heflinger 《Journal of child and family studies》2005,14(1):43-56
The aim of this study was to examine differences in responses to the Caregiver Strain Questionnaire (CGSQ) between African American and White caregivers of children with emotional and behavioral challenges. Significant item- and scale-level differences were detected across groups with African Americans consistently reporting less strain. We examined whether these differences were more likely due to nonequivalent measurement than to real differences in the experience of caregiver strain. Confirmatory factor analysis showed that the model fit the data well for both racial groups, but there were some differences in structural components. Internal consistency was equivalent across the groups. In an examination of criterion validity, regression analyses showed that African American caregivers experienced a slower rate of increase in objective caregiver strain at a given increase in child internalizing problems. No other race differences were found in the regression analyses. In general, we conclude that the CGSQ can be useful for detecting caregiver strain and identifying family support needs for both White and African American caregivers. Differences across groups in reports of caregiver strain, however, call for more research on racial differences in the impact on the family of caring for a child with emotional and behavioral disorders. 相似文献
802.
Williams JK Else 'R Hishinuma ES Goebert DA Chang JY Andrade NN Nishimura ST 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2005,11(1):41-56
A confirmatory model integrating Japanese ethnicity, cultural identity, and depression was developed (N = 140). The model incorporated the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D), Major Life Events Scale, and Japanese Cultural Scale. Japanese American adolescents scored higher on the Japanese Cultural Scale and reported fewer depressive symptoms on the CES-D total and on 2 of the 3 CES-D factors than part-Japanese American adolescents. Predictors for depression were being Japanese American vs. part-Japanese American, female gender, and culturally intensified events. A significant interaction of behavior by self-identification was noted. The model had good overall fit and suggested that the formation of cultural identity may contribute to depressive symptoms experienced by adolescents, particularly adolescents of mixed heritage. 相似文献
803.
This study researches the input of the cerebral occipital and temporal cortex in the analysis of facial configuration and expressive characteristics. Analysis is based on the construction of a spherical model for the differentiation of schematically presented faces with quantitatively altering curvature of the mouth and brows. The model is designed using the method of multidimensional scaling of the dissimilarity judgments between stimuli (faces) and the amplitude of evoked potentials of differences (EPD) between abrupt stimulus changes recorded from the occipital and posterior temporal cortex. Analysis of the structure of the spherical model of facial differentiation depending on the electrode site and the latency of the EPD component within the duration of 120-240 ms has demonstrated that the activity of the occipital and posterior temporal cortex of the right hemisphere is associated with the emotional characteristics of the presented face, whereas facial configuration is reflected in the activation of both posterior temporal cortex and the occipital cortex of the left hemisphere. At all electrode sites maximum information of the emotional expression and configuration is represented in inter-peak amplitude P120-N180. With increasing latency there is increased distortion of the structure of differences in the spherical model of schematically presented faces, which is interpreted as an attenuation of electrical activity associated with the analysis of the emotional expression, which occurs more rapidly than configuration analysis. 相似文献
804.
Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomical basis? 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
Craig AD 《Trends in cognitive sciences》2005,9(12):566-571
There is considerable psychophysiological evidence to indicate that the left and right halves of the human forebrain differentially associate with particular emotions and affective traits. A neurobiological model is needed. Here I propose that forebrain emotional asymmetry is anatomically based on an asymmetrical representation of homeostatic activity that originates from asymmetries in the peripheral autonomic nervous system. This proposal builds on recent evidence indicating that lateralized, higher-order re-representations of homeostatic sensory activity provide a foundation for subjective human feelings. It can subsume differing views of emotion and the forebrain because it suggests that emotions are organized according to the fundamental principle of autonomic opponency for the management of physical and mental energy. 相似文献
805.
Miles JN 《Behavior research methods》2005,37(4):672-676
This article presents a method for using Microsoft (MS) Excel for confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). CFA is often seen as
an impenetrable technique, and thus, when it is taught, there is frequently little explanation of the mechanisms or underlying
calculations. The aim of this article is to demonstrate that this is not the case; it is relatively straightforward to produce
a spreadsheet in MS Excel that can carry out simple CFA. It is possible, with few or no programming skills, to effectively
program a CFA analysis and, thus, to gain insight into the workings of the procedure. 相似文献
806.
807.
808.
809.
Throughout the U.S., state laws require professionals who work with children to report cases of suspected child abuse to child protection services. Both practically and conceptually, however, significant problems arise from a lack of clarity regarding the threshold that has been set for reporting. Specifically, there is no consensus as to what constitutes reasonable suspicion, and little direction for how mandated reporters should gauge their legal and professional responsibilities when they harbor suspicion. In this paper we outline the context of the problem, discuss the nature and scope of its conceptual underpinnings, and offer recommendations for moving towards a concrete, practical solution. 相似文献
810.
Two diary studies investigated the prevalence of memory conjunction errors for real-life events. In both studies, participants completed detailed diary pages over the course of several weeks. Participants in both diary studies committed memory conjunction errors on a later recognition memory test. In the second diary study participants also made remember/know judgements. For a large proportion of their memory conjunction errors participants indicated that they "remembered" the event occurring in that context. These diary studies demonstrate that memory conjunction errors do occur for autobiographical memories, and these errors can seem compelling. 相似文献