全文获取类型
收费全文 | 239篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
240篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 30篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 3篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1954年 | 1篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
1948年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有240条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Levy BR Ryall AL Pilver CE Sheridan PL Wei JY Hausdorff JM 《Anxiety, stress, and coping》2008,21(1):85-93
Although it has been shown that White elders are vulnerable to the influence of age stereotypes, it was not known whether this effect applied to African American elders. In the present study, African American elders were randomly assigned to negative or positive implicit-age-stereotype groups. Compared to participants in the positive age-stereotype group, those in the negative age-stereotype group demonstrated significantly more elevated cardiovascular response to stress, as measured by blood pressure and heart rate following mental challenges. These results suggest that negative age stereotypes generate a susceptibility to stress among African American elders, whereas positive age may provide them with a defense against this stress. 相似文献
102.
103.
104.
Joyce Ehrlinger E. Ashby Plant Richard P. Eibach Corey J. Columb Joanna L. Goplen Jonathan W. Kunstman David A. Butz 《Political psychology》2011,32(1):131-146
Leading up to the 2008 U.S. election, pundits wondered whether Whites, particularly in Southern states, were ready to vote for a Black president. The present paper explores how a common Southern symbol—the Confederate flag—impacted willingness to vote for Barack Obama. We predicted that exposure to the Confederate flag would activate negativity toward Blacks and result in lowered willingness to vote for Obama. As predicted, participants primed with the Confederate flag reported less willingness to vote for Obama than those primed with a neutral symbol. The flag did not affect willingness to vote for White candidates. In a second study, participants primed with the Confederate flag evaluated a hypothetical Black target more negatively than controls. These results suggest that exposure to the Confederate flag results in more negative judgments of Black targets. As such, the prevalence of this flag in the South may have contributed to a reticence for some to vote for Obama because of his race. 相似文献
105.
The purpose of this research is to test the importance of temporal trends within absenteeism data for males and females. The data support previous findings of higher absenteeism rates for women when compared to men. The data also indicate the importance of temporal trends as suggested by Dansereau, Alutto, and Markham (1978). Conclusions are drawn concerning the use of absence rates as dependent variables. 相似文献
106.
Significant job-relatedness was found for a posttraining job knowledge test criterion using an application of Lawshe's content validity method. The aide test was used as a criterion to assess the predictive validity of a vocabulary test and a civil service test with samples of black ( N = 43) and white ( N = 62) psychiatric aides. Significant validities were found on both tests, but a vocabulary test proved to be the better predictor of the criterion in both samples. The obtained validities were discussed in terms of differential validity, test fairness, and sample size. This study demonstrated that a content validity method could be applied to criteria as well as selection tests. It was concluded that content validity methods may be able to help solve the problem of criterion relevance in validation research by providing quantitative evidence of the job-relatedness of criteria. 相似文献
107.
JOSÉ PARRA CARDONA PH.D. KENDAL HOLTROP M.A. DAVID CÓRDOVA JR. M.A. ANA ROCIO ESCOBAR-CHEW B.S. SHEENA HORSFORD B.S. LISA TAMS MSW † FRANCISCO A. VILLARRUEL PH.D. GRACIELA VILLALOBOS‡ BRIAN DATES‡ JAMES C. ANTHONY PH.D. § HIRAM E. FITZGERALD PH.D. ¶ 《Family process》2009,48(2):211-231
Despite the unique and challenging circumstances confronting Latino immigrant families, debate still exists as to the need to culturally adapt evidence-based interventions for dissemination with this population. Following the grounded theory approach, the current qualitative investigation utilized focus group interviews with 83 Latino immigrant parents to explore the relevance of culturally adapting an evidence-based parenting intervention to be disseminated within this population. Findings from this study indicate that Latino immigrant parents want to participate in a culturally adapted parenting intervention as long as it is culturally relevant, respectful, and responsive to their life experiences. Research results also suggest that the parenting skills participants seek to enhance are among those commonly targeted by evidence-based parenting interventions. This study contributes to the cultural adaptation/fidelity balance debate by highlighting the necessity of exploring ways to develop culturally adapted interventions characterized by high cultural relevance, as well as high fidelity to the core components that have established efficacy for evidence-based parenting interventions. 相似文献
108.
The present study tested diffusion models of processing in the flanker task, in which participants identify a target that is flanked by items that indicate the same (congruent) or opposite response (incongruent). Single- and dual-process flanker models were implemented in a diffusion-model framework and tested against data from experiments that manipulated response bias, speed/accuracy tradeoffs, attentional focus, and stimulus configuration. There was strong mimcry among the models, and each captured the main trends in the data for the standard conditions. However, when more complex conditions were used, a single-process spotlight model captured qualitative and quantitative patterns that the dual-process models could not. Since the single-process model provided the best balance of fit quality and parsimony, the results indicate that processing in the simple versions of the flanker task is better described by gradual rather than discrete narrowing of attention. 相似文献
109.
MYLAN ENGEL JR. 《The Southern journal of philosophy》2012,50(1):50-74
This paper defends a coherentist approach to moral epistemology. In “The Immorality of Eating Meat” (2000), I offer a coherentist consistency argument to show that our own beliefs rationally commit us to the immorality of eating meat. Elsewhere, I use our own beliefs as premises to argue that we have positive duties to assist the poor (2004) and to argue that biomedical animal experimentation is wrong (2012). The present paper explores whether this consistency‐based coherentist approach of grounding particular moral judgments on beliefs we already hold, with no appeal to moral theory, is a legitimate way of doing practical ethics. I argue (i) that grounding particular moral judgments on our core moral convictions and other core nonmoral beliefs is a legitimate way to justify moral judgments, (ii) that these moral judgments possess as much epistemic justification and have as much claim to objectivity as moral judgments grounded on particular ethical theories, and (iii) that this internalistic coherentist method of grounding moral judgments is more likely to result in behavioral guidance than traditional theory‐based approaches to practical ethics. By way of illustrating the approach, I briefly recapitulate my consistency‐based argument for ethical vegetarianism. I then defend the coherentist approach implicit in the argument against a number of potentially fatal metatheoretical attacks. 相似文献
110.
Heather M. Kleider-Offutt Alesha D. Bond Sarah E. Williams Corey J. Bohil 《Memory & cognition》2018,46(5):716-728
Prior research indicates that stereotypical Black faces (e.g., wide nose, full lips: Afrocentric) are often associated with crime and violence. The current study investigated whether stereotypical faces may bias the interpretation of facial expression to seem threatening. Stimuli were prerated by face type (stereotypical, nonstereotypical) and expression (neutral, threatening). Later in a forced-choice task, different participants categorized face stimuli as stereotypical or not and threatening or not. Regardless of prerated expression, stereotypical faces were judged as more threatening than were nonstereotypical faces. These findings were supported using computational models based on general recognition theory (GRT), indicating that decision boundaries were more biased toward the threatening response for stereotypical faces than for nonstereotypical faces. GRT analysis also indicated that perception of face stereotypicality and emotional expression are dependent, both across categories and within individual categories. Higher perceived stereotypicality predicts higher perception of threat, and, conversely, higher ratings of threat predict higher perception of stereotypicality. Implications for racial face-type bias influencing perception and decision-making in a variety of social and professional contexts are discussed. 相似文献