首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   42477篇
  免费   1686篇
  国内免费   16篇
  2020年   489篇
  2019年   613篇
  2018年   815篇
  2017年   890篇
  2016年   905篇
  2015年   649篇
  2014年   782篇
  2013年   3377篇
  2012年   1429篇
  2011年   1430篇
  2010年   926篇
  2009年   877篇
  2008年   1235篇
  2007年   1259篇
  2006年   1124篇
  2005年   992篇
  2004年   931篇
  2003年   881篇
  2002年   911篇
  2001年   1320篇
  2000年   1271篇
  1999年   995篇
  1998年   421篇
  1997年   406篇
  1996年   385篇
  1993年   390篇
  1992年   823篇
  1991年   730篇
  1990年   780篇
  1989年   682篇
  1988年   707篇
  1987年   674篇
  1986年   685篇
  1985年   631篇
  1984年   576篇
  1983年   536篇
  1980年   390篇
  1979年   658篇
  1978年   442篇
  1977年   384篇
  1975年   515篇
  1974年   586篇
  1973年   602篇
  1972年   459篇
  1971年   455篇
  1970年   406篇
  1969年   470篇
  1968年   534篇
  1967年   500篇
  1966年   396篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
44.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
In Experiment 1, rats were trained on a discrimination between rubber- and sandpaper-covered arms of a maze after one group had been pre-exposed to these intra-maze cues. Pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made easier by adding further discriminative stimuli, when it now significantly retarded learning. In Experiment 2, rats were trained on an extra-maze spatial discrimination, again after one group, but not another, had been pre-exposed to the extra-maze landmarks. Here too, pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made substantially easier by arranging that the two arms between which rats had to choose were always separated by 135°. The results of both experiments can be explained by supposing that perceptual learning depends on the presence of features common to S+ and S-.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号