首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   51139篇
  免费   1362篇
  国内免费   25篇
  52526篇
  2020年   353篇
  2019年   438篇
  2018年   3942篇
  2017年   3257篇
  2016年   2814篇
  2015年   619篇
  2014年   610篇
  2013年   2529篇
  2012年   1544篇
  2011年   3298篇
  2010年   3026篇
  2009年   2114篇
  2008年   2499篇
  2007年   2966篇
  2006年   805篇
  2005年   891篇
  2004年   824篇
  2003年   721篇
  2002年   687篇
  2001年   1088篇
  2000年   1111篇
  1999年   802篇
  1998年   345篇
  1997年   319篇
  1995年   312篇
  1992年   627篇
  1991年   594篇
  1990年   571篇
  1989年   546篇
  1988年   542篇
  1987年   481篇
  1986年   482篇
  1985年   531篇
  1984年   440篇
  1983年   402篇
  1982年   314篇
  1979年   474篇
  1978年   331篇
  1977年   302篇
  1976年   321篇
  1975年   361篇
  1974年   437篇
  1973年   464篇
  1972年   401篇
  1971年   347篇
  1970年   318篇
  1969年   337篇
  1968年   416篇
  1967年   370篇
  1966年   357篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Four experiments are described which aim to distinguish the relative contributions of measures of semantic distance based on formal and normative criteria. Experiment I replicates a previous finding by Collins and Quillian (1969) that sentence confirmation RTs support a hierarchically-organised memory model. Experiments II and III minimised the role of syntactic processes and examined the times taken to “see the relationship” between pairs of concepts. The results also supported a hierarchical model but cast doubt upon the formal distinction between superset and property relationships. Experiment IV showed similar results using only property relationships of the “has” form. Multiple regression analyses of the data indicate that “number of intervening links” is a more consistent predictor of RT than “associative” measures of semantic relatendness and confirm strong linearity effects consistent with a hierarchical model of storage.  相似文献   
3.
4.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号