全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1441篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1501篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 102篇 |
2012年 | 42篇 |
2011年 | 58篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 45篇 |
2007年 | 41篇 |
2006年 | 44篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 48篇 |
2003年 | 41篇 |
2002年 | 46篇 |
2001年 | 45篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 20篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 19篇 |
1971年 | 21篇 |
1969年 | 18篇 |
1967年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1501条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
82.
CULTURAL ADJUSTMENT OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
83.
The asymmetric dominance effect (ADE) occurs when the introduction of a partially dominated decoy option increases the choice share of its dominating alternative. The ADE is a violation of regularity and the constant-ratio rule, which are two derivations of the independence of irrelevant alternatives axiom, a core tenant of rational choice. The ADE is one of the most widely reported human choice phenomena, leading researchers to probe its origins by studying a variety of non-human species. We examined the ADE in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), a species that displays many other decision biases. In Experiment 1, we used a touchscreen method to elicit choice-based preferences for food rewards in asymmetrically dominated choice sets. In Experiments 2 and 3, we distinguished between different types of judgments and used a free selection task to elicit consumption-based preferences for juice rewards. However, we found no evidence for the ADE through violations of regularity or the constant-ratio rule, despite the similarity of our stimuli to other human and non-human experiments. While these results appear to conflict with existing literature on the ADE in non-human species, we point out methodological differences—notably, the distinction between value-based and perception-based stimuli—that have led to a collection of phenomena that are difficult to understand under a unitary theoretical framework. In particular, we highlight key differences between the human and non-human research and provide a series of steps that researchers could take to better understand the ADE. 相似文献
84.
Diedrichsen J Ivry RB Cohen A Danziger S 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(1):113-126
Four experiments were conducted using a flanker task with 1 distractor appearing either on the left or right side of a central target. Responses were made on a keyboard aligned parallel to the displays. A larger flanker effect was obtained when the distractor was on the same side as the response. Two factors account for this asymmetry. First, when the flanker and target are identical, the 2 form a group that is assigned a spatial tag, creating a form of the Simon effect on the basis of the compatibility between the response keys and the group. Second, preparation of a lateralized response appears to entail a shift of visual attention in the corresponding direction, thus enhancing processing of the flanker on the response side. Consistent with the 2nd hypothesis, participants were more likely to correctly recognize letters that were briefly presented at the distractor position on the same side as the response. 相似文献
85.
Research on the benefits of social support groups has been inconclusive. One reason is that individual differences in intervention responses have rarely been examined. The authors determined the extent to which individual difference variables moderated the effects of an information-based educational group and an emotion-focused peer discussion group on the mental and physical functioning of women with breast cancer (n = 230). The authors administered the SF-36 (S.E. Ware, K.K. Snow, M. Kosinski, & B. Gandek, 1993), a multidimensional quality of life instrument, pre- and postintervention. Educational groups showed greater benefits on the physical functioning of women who started the study with more difficulties compared with less difficulties (e.g., lacked support or fewer personal resources). Peer discussion groups were helpful for women who lacked support from their partners or physicians but harmful for women who had high levels of support. Implications of these results for clinical interventions are discussed. 相似文献
86.
G.A. Cohen 《The Journal of Ethics》2000,4(1-2):1-26
Many people, including many egalitarian political philosophers, professa belief in equality while enjoying high incomes of which they devotevery little to egalitarian purposes. The article critically examinesways of resolving the putative inconsistency in the stance of thesepeople, in particular, that favouring an egalitarian society has noimplications for behaviour in an unequal one; that what's bad aboutinequality is a social division that philanthropy cannot reduce; thatprivate action cannot ensure that others have good lives; that privateaction can only achieve a ``drop in the ocean'; that private effortis not called for, since justice is a matter for the state to enforce;that private effort cannot remove the fundamental injustice, whichis inequality of power; and that private effort involves an unreasonablylarge psychological burden. 相似文献
87.
88.
Much coping research has been conducted comparing members of different ethnic groups using discrete racial classifications. However, the past two decades have seen the construct of ethnicity evolve into a complex variable that must be assessed in a more comprehensive way. This study explored how ethnicity, a discrete variable, and the continuous variables of a person's ethnic identity and self-construal contribute to the use of particular coping strategies across various situations. One hundred twelve seventh graders (67 African Americans and 45 Caucasian Americans) from three suburban middle schools completed questionnaires assessing ethnic identity, self-construal, and coping strategies for medical, test, and social criticism stressors. Results supported the hypothesis that ethnicity as a discrete variable is not associated with coping, but that ethnic identity and self-construal are. It was also found that high scores on the ethnic identity and self-construal scales were indicative of more positive psychological adjustment. Implications for future research and methodological considerations are discussed. 相似文献
89.
Ted Cohen 《Philosophical Studies》2002,108(3):339-340
Volume Contents
contents Volume 108, 2002 相似文献90.
Rats were exposed to three-trial series consisting of reinforced (R) trials and one nonreinforced (N) trial in a fixed order, RRN and RNR (Experiments 1 and 2) or NRR and RRN (Experiment 3), on extended visually distinct runways in a T-maze. When initially presented with the same sequence on each series in a session (separate presentations) with the same runway on all trials within a series (Experiments 1 and 3), all the rats developed slower running speeds on N than on R trials. When a runway was sometimes changed between the first and next two trials during separate presentations training (Experiment 2) or both sequences were later intermixed within each session in each experiment, only rats exposed to each sequence on a specific runway maintained these serial running patterns. Rats displayed serial running patterns on a test RNN sequence similar to that on the RNR sequence (Experiment 2), as would be predicted by an intertrial association model of serial pattern learning (Capaldi & Molina, 1979), but responded on test RRR and NRN sequences (Experiment 3) as would be predicted by an ordinal-trial-tag/intratrial association model (Burns, Wiley, & Payne, 1986). Results from test series of free-choice trials in Experiments 1 and 2 failed to support a prediction of the intratrial association model that these rats would integrate RRN and RNR sequences. Rather than always selecting a baited runway on both the second and the third free-choice trials, the rats only selected a baited runway on the third trial on the basis of their choice on the second trial, as would be predicted by the intertrial association model. Only after experiencing all possible outcome sequences during forced-choice training in Experiment 3 did these rats predominantly select a baited runway on every free-choice trial. 相似文献