首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   264篇
  免费   3篇
  2023年   4篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   5篇
  2014年   6篇
  2013年   27篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   6篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   4篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   8篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   4篇
  1968年   7篇
  1967年   5篇
  1966年   4篇
  1960年   2篇
  1957年   2篇
  1941年   2篇
排序方式: 共有267条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
Consider any scoring procedure for determining whether an examinee knows the answer to a test item. Letx i = 1 if a correct decision is made about whether the examinee knows the ith item; otherwisex i = 0. Thek out ofn reliability of a test isρ k = Pr (Σx i k). That is,ρ k is the probability of making at leastk correct decisions for a typical (randomly sampled) examinee. This paper proposes an approximation ofρ k that can be estimated with an answer-until-correct test. The paper also suggests a scoring procedure that might be used whenρ k is judged to be too small under a conventional scoring rule where it is decided an examinee knows if and only if the correct response is given.  相似文献   
62.
The present study presents evidence that young children's comprehension of the locatives ‘in’, ‘on’, and ‘under’ is, at least in part, contextually determined. Children aged 1;6–3;0 were given tasks with verbal instructions which were either contextually congruent or incongruent. The results were interpreted in terms of the non-linguistic as well as linguistic strategies apparently used to interpret speech. The results and interpretation are in contrast to those of earlier research.  相似文献   
63.
Two field experiments tested the hypothesis that a bystander's increased responsibility to act increases the likelihood of his helping the victim of an emergency. In Experiment 1, an individual asked or did not ask the bystander to protect his property in his absence. In Experiment 2, the presence/absence of an unattentive confederate was varied orthogonally to the request/no request manipulation. The results of both experiments indicated that bystanders who received a prior request for protective assistance felt more personally responsible for protecting the individual's property and were more likely to prevent a theft of that property than were bystanders who received no request. The presence of a confederate in Experiment 2 decreased bystanders' felt responsibility and their willingness to intervene on the victim's behalf. The results were interpreted as support for the "felt responsibility" proposition of the Latané and Darley (1970) model of bystander intervention.  相似文献   
64.
Two experiments investigated characteristic generalized imitation procedures on a nonimitative successive visual discrimination task. In Experiment I, no discriminative behavior was obtained though a number of procedures designed to enhance discrimination were employed. The introduction of a differential modeling procedure after nine or 10 sessions of nondifferential performance was ineffective in producing differential responding for three of four Ss. In the second experiment, the differential modeling procedure was introduced as an initial manipulation and was successful in establishing discriminative performance in all Ss. After several sessions of differential responding, the observation of a model performing nondifferentially was relatively ineffective in altering the pattern of responding. These results suggest (1) that modeling may be functional in altering a S's behavior depending on what point in the procedures the manipulation is introduced and (2) that certain procedural variables may define a functional response class characterized by the failure to develop discriminative responding.  相似文献   
65.
66.
The present study tested three assumptions often made by smoking prevention researchers and practitioners regarding the nature and causes of smoking prevalence estimates. Specifically, it is often assumed that (a) adolescent prevalence estimates of all levels of smoking are inflated, (b) youths at different stages of smoking onset all overestimate smoking prevalence, and (c) the predictors of these estimates are the same for youths at different stages of smoking onset. Measures of smoking behavior based on self-reports using a bogus pipeline procedure, estimates of cigarette smoking prevalence (trying behavior and weekly use), and reports of smoking in significant others were obtained by the same youths when they were in eighth and ninth grades. Results indicated that, taken over all youths, estimates of trying cigarettes were accurate, whereas estimates of weekly use were inflated. When responders were defined by their stage of smoking onset, either as a nonsmoker, trier, or regular smoker, analyses revealed that triers were accurate regarding prevalence estimates of trying, whereas non-smokers made gross underestimations and regular smokers made overestimations of trying. All groups of youths overestimated prevalence of weekly smoking. Analyses of variance revealed that both youths' smoking status and interpersonal smoking environment additively predicted prevalence estimates, and estimates of smoking prevalence were relatively higher in youths who began to smoke, or increased their levels of smoking, within the next year. Thus, previous assumptions were partially confirmed. Future research and prevention programming should focus more on regular smoking as the focus of normative smoking distortion effects and correction efforts.  相似文献   
67.
Medical decision making often utilizes subjective observations to arrive at concrete judgments. The decisions frequently affect who receives scarce medical treatments and, thus, who lives or dies. In this paper, a model health status index is described. It is specific for the problem of choosing patients for hemodialysis or transplantation. Such a health status index may be designed for any medical decision involving such issues as drug treatment priorities, identification of salvageable patients, and selection of patients for scarce medical treatment. This index (1) incorporates a physician's own medical criteria and values, (2) can be modified as the data base improves, (3) assures consistency from decision to decision, and (4) can be developed and used without the help of a mathematician or computer.  相似文献   
68.
In water striders or other animals that communicate with vibratory or oscillatory signals, surface wave signals can be recorded, analyzed in digital form by computer, then played back accurately by oscillating a magnet, glued to the animal, with electromagnetic fluctuations from a coil driven by the computer. Signals can be played with great flexibility through a normally behaving animal, in laboratory or field.  相似文献   
69.
70.
This study examines the relative emphasis placed upon social status and prestige in the selection of an occupation and program of study by students in the community-junior college. The following results were noted: (1) Students enrolled in college transfer programs stressed prestige in the selection of an occupation to a greater extent than did those in technical and semi-professional programs. (2) When the students in the sample were grouped according to occupational aspiration level, a positive correlation was found to exist between occupational aspiration level and prestige emphasis. (3) No significant relationship was found to exist between the social status of students and prestige emphasis in the selection of an occupation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号