全文获取类型
收费全文 | 460篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
464篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 41篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 17篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1955年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有464条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
Immigration, Acculturation, and Adaptation 总被引:45,自引:0,他引:45
John W. Berry 《Psychologie appliquee》1997,46(1):5-34
82.
Carol K. Sigelman Carol J. Berry Katharine A. Wiles 《Journal of applied social psychology》1984,14(6):530-548
In a survey of 504 college students examining predictors of violence in heterosexual relationships, over half of both men and women had committed at least one physically violent act, and men more often than women reported having been the victims of such acts. Most respondents who reported some experience with violence had both committed and received it, were involved in relatively few different types of violence, and first experienced violence when a relationship had moved beyond the casual dating stage. Modest associations between physical violence and sexual aggression were uncovered. In a series of discriminant analyses, men who abused their partners were not readily distinguished from men who did not, but tended to be young, low in family income, traditional in attitudes toward women, abused as children, currently living with a woman, and from Appalachian areas. Women who abused were more readily discriminated and scored low in social desirability, were abused as children, and were from non-Appalachian areas. Men who were abused were likely to be living with a woman and tended to be low in family income; similarly, cohabitation was related among women to being a target of violence, as were having been abused as a child and scoring low in social desirability. Findings are related to those of other studies of dating abuse, as well as to the family violence and aggression literatures. 相似文献
83.
J.W. Berry 《International journal of psychology》1989,24(6):721-735
There is a tension between two research traditions in cross-cultural psychology: working intensively within a single culture in order to understand indigenous psychological phenomena and how they are related to cultural context; and working comparatively across cultures in order to understand broad patterns of relationships between behavioural and cultural variables. This tension can be resolved, and the two approaches integrated, by the adoption of the emic and etic concepts of Pike, and by the elaboration of a set of concrete research steps rooted in these concepts. This paper outlines a conceptual and operational framework for the pursuit of both the indigenous and comparative goals, using examples from research on intelligence and attitudes. 相似文献
84.
85.
86.
87.
Josephine J. Cock Dianne C. Berry E. A. Gaffan 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1994,47(4):1015-1034
This paper reports three experiments that examine the role of similarity processing in McGeorge and Burton's (1990) incidental learning task. In the experiments subjects performed a distractor task involving four-digit number strings, all of which conformed to a simple hidden rule. They were then given a forced-choice memory test in which they were presented with pairs of strings and were led to believe that one string of each pair had appeared in the prior learning phase. Although this was not the case, one string of each pair did conform to the hidden rule. Experiment 1 showed that, as in the McGeorge and Burton study, subjects were significantly more likely to select test strings that conformed to the hidden rule. However, additional analyses suggested that rather than having implicitly abstracted the rule, subjects may have been selecting strings that were in some way similar to those seen during the learning phase. Experiments 2 and 3 were designed to try to separate out effects due to similarity from those due to implicit rule abstraction. It was found that the results were more consistent with a similarity-based model than implicit rule abstraction per se. 相似文献
88.
89.
Four experiments are reported. These experiments compare the effects of two different forms of machine advice on performance
on a complex search task. The task involves determining which of a set of factories is responsible for polluting a river by
testing the river for the presence or absence of various pollutants. Subjects either receive a suggestion as to which pollutant
they should test for on each go, or following testing for a pollutant they are told which of the factories could possibly
be polluting the river at that stage, or they receive a combination of the two forms of advice. The experiments show that
suggesting to subjects which pollutant they should test for on each go is of no benefit, whereas providing a list of possible
factory names after testing for each pollutant significantly benefits performance. Only subjects who receive both types of
advice simultaneously, however, maintain an improved performance level when the advice is withdrawn.
This research was supported by the Alvey Directorate. Mr. E.C. Portman of ICL, West Gorton, acted as “industrial uncle” to
the project. 相似文献
90.
M J Strube J M Berry C L Lott R Fogelman G Steinhart S Moergen L Davison 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1986,51(1):170-180
Past research has established clear behavioral differences between Type A and B individuals. The purpose of our research was to examine how these behavioral differences are represented in the self-definitions of Type As and Bs. We investigated the existence of Type A and B self-schemata by using two tasks designed to measure the influence of these hypothetical structures on speed of processing and memory interference. During an initial task, Type As and Bs made self-relevant decisions (like me, not like me) in response to trait adjectives previously scaled as Type A, Type B, or neutral in content. Reaction times for the decisions were measured, and results indicated that both Type As and Bs made faster decisions for schema-compatible responses than for schema-incompatible responses. On a second task, Type As and Bs were tested for recognition memory after they attempted to memorize half of the aforementioned trait list. Memory errors were examined and indicated that Type As and Bs made more errors that were compatible with their respective self-schemata. Taken together, these results indicate that a Type A and B distinction forms a reliable organizing framework for the self-definitions of Type As and Bs. The existence of stable cognitive structures that parallel the behavioral differences between Type As and Bs has important implications for both theory and application. 相似文献