全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17029篇 |
免费 | 355篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
17385篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 243篇 |
2018年 | 274篇 |
2017年 | 234篇 |
2016年 | 280篇 |
2015年 | 182篇 |
2014年 | 278篇 |
2013年 | 1332篇 |
2012年 | 475篇 |
2011年 | 508篇 |
2010年 | 308篇 |
2009年 | 326篇 |
2008年 | 441篇 |
2007年 | 463篇 |
2006年 | 446篇 |
2005年 | 369篇 |
2004年 | 390篇 |
2003年 | 346篇 |
2002年 | 351篇 |
2001年 | 462篇 |
2000年 | 522篇 |
1999年 | 350篇 |
1998年 | 212篇 |
1997年 | 160篇 |
1996年 | 172篇 |
1995年 | 169篇 |
1994年 | 156篇 |
1992年 | 280篇 |
1991年 | 289篇 |
1990年 | 298篇 |
1989年 | 282篇 |
1988年 | 274篇 |
1987年 | 240篇 |
1986年 | 254篇 |
1985年 | 266篇 |
1984年 | 195篇 |
1983年 | 211篇 |
1982年 | 160篇 |
1980年 | 157篇 |
1979年 | 251篇 |
1978年 | 220篇 |
1975年 | 216篇 |
1974年 | 245篇 |
1973年 | 268篇 |
1972年 | 226篇 |
1971年 | 212篇 |
1970年 | 163篇 |
1969年 | 196篇 |
1968年 | 248篇 |
1967年 | 240篇 |
1966年 | 209篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
821.
822.
823.
Schubö A Schlaghecken F Meinecke C 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2001,27(4):919-931
Although traditionally texture segmentation has been regarded as an automatic, preattentive process, participants confronted with texture segmentation in experimental settings (i.e., with brief presentation time and subsequent masking) are initially unable to perform the task. According to perceptual learning concepts, participants must learn to fine-tune their sensory channels before perception improves under restricted viewing conditions. The present article proposes an alternative perspective that emphasizes the role of the mask. Four experiments showed that the amount of observed learning depends on the structural and temporal homogeneity or heterogeneity of the mask. The authors suggest that learning consists of separating the task-relevant signal stemming from the texture from the task-irrelevant signal of the mask and of ignoring the mask. 相似文献
824.
825.
In our study newborn infants were presented with lists of lexical and grammatical words prepared from natural maternal speech. The results show that newborns are able to categorically discriminate these sets of words based on a constellation of perceptual cues that distinguish them. This general ability to detect and categorically discriminate sets of words on the basis of multiple acoustic and phonological cues may provide a perceptual base that can help older infants bootstrap into the acquisition of grammatical categories and syntactic structure. 相似文献
826.
827.
828.
Studies of bimanual coordination have found that only two stable relative phases (0 degree and 180 degrees) are produced when a participant rhythmically moves two joints in different limbs at the same frequency. Increasing the frequency of oscillation causes an increase in relative phase variability in both of these phase modes. However, relative phasing at 180 degrees is more variable than relative phasing at 0 degree, and when the frequency of oscillation reaches a critical frequency, a transition to 0 degree occurs. These results have been replicated when 2 people have coordinated their respective limb movements using vision. This inspired us to investigate the visual perception of relative phase. In Experiment 1, recordings of human interlimb oscillations exhibiting different frequencies, mean relative phases, and different amounts of phase variability were used to generate computer displays of spheres oscillating either side to side in a frontoparallel plane or in depth. Participants judged the stability of relative phase. Judgments covaried with phase variability only when the mean phase was 0 degree or 180 degrees. Otherwise, judgments covaried with mean relative phase, even after extensive instruction and demonstration. In Experiment 2, mean relative phase and phase variability were manipulated independently via simulations, and participants were trained to perceive phase variability in testing sessions in which mean phase was held constant. The results of Experiment 1 were replicated. The HKB model was fitted to mean judgment standard deviations. 相似文献
829.
Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion 总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16
Extraversion has two central characteristics: (1) interpersonal engagement, which consists of affiliation (enjoying and valuing close interpersonal bonds, being warm and affectionate) and agency (being socially dominant, enjoying leadership roles, being assertive, being exhibitionistic, and having a sense of potency in accomplishing goals) and (2) impulsivity, which emerges from the interaction of extraversion and a second, independent trait (constraint). Agency is a more general motivational disposition that includes dominance, ambition, mastery, efficacy, and achievement. Positive affect (a combination of positive feelings and motivation) is closely associated with extraversion. Extraversion is accordingly based on positive incentive motivation. Parallels between extraversion (particularly its agency component) and a mammalian behavioral approach system based on positive incentive motivation implicate a neuroanatomical network and modulatory neurotransmitters in the processing of incentive motivation. A corticolimbic-striatal-thalamic network (1) integrates the salient incentive context in the medial orbital cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus; (2) encodes the intensity of incentive stimuli in a motive circuit composed of the nucleus accumbens, ventral pallidum, and ventral tegmental area dopamine projection system; and (3) creates an incentive motivational state that can be transmitted to the motor system. Individual differences in the functioning of this network arise from functional variation in the ventral tegmental area dopamine projections, which are directly involved in coding the intensity of incentive motivation. The animal evidence suggests that there are three neurodevelopmental sources of individual differences in dopamine: genetic, "experience-expectant," and "experience-dependent." Individual differences in dopamine promote variation in the heterosynaptic plasticity that enhances the connection between incentive context and incentive motivation and behavior. Our psychobiological threshold model explains the effects of individual differences in dopamine transmission on behavior, and their relation to personality traits is discussed. 相似文献
830.
Marcus RF 《Adolescence》1999,34(133):33-46
One hundred sixty-three young adolescents (72 males, 91 females) were administered a 23-item self-report inventory measuring antisocial behaviors. Results were analyzed separately for males and females. Exploratory factor analysis showed antisocial behavior to be multidimensional and to have different factor structures for males and females. The three factors accounting for more than two-thirds of the variance in antisocial behavior are discussed in terms of specialization in drug/alcohol use and violence. 相似文献