全文获取类型
收费全文 | 996篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
专业分类
1055篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 32篇 |
2018年 | 39篇 |
2017年 | 58篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 36篇 |
2013年 | 112篇 |
2012年 | 74篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 34篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 36篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 26篇 |
2003年 | 28篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1055条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Claudia J. Haferkamp 《Current Psychology》1994,13(3):248-262
Relationships among relationship beliefs, self-monitoring and conflict behavior were examined in 74 married couples. It was
predicted that dysfunctional relationship beliefs would be negatively correlated with marital satisfaction and that high self-monitoring
spouses would endorse more dysfunctional relationship beliefs and display more disagreeing and leadership conflict behavior.
Dysfunctional beliefs were negatively correlated with satisfaction but low and high self-monitors did not differ in their
conflict behavior. Discriminant analyses indicated that marital distress was a function of endorsement of dysfunctional relationship
beliefs; decreased satisfaction with decisions; increased conflict relevance; and increased impression management reflected
in high self-monitoring orientations among husbands and wives’ beliefs that their husbands were not behaving “normally.” The
contribution of gender and impression management to marital distress is discussed.
This study was funded by a University of Wisconsin-Madison faculty research grant awarded to the author. 相似文献
12.
13.
A “reinterpretation of inconsistent cognitions” explanation of the previous finding that thought about an object tends to polarize attitudes toward that object was tested. Under the assumption that ambiguity facilitates reinterpretation, ambiguity of inconsistent information and opportunity for thought were manipulated. Consistent with expectations, (a) thought resulted in attitude polarization and this effect was increased with ambiguity (p < .01) and (b) evaluation (interpretation) of the inconsistent information varied as a function of thought (p < .05) and ambiguity (p < .01). 相似文献
14.
15.
16.
In study 1, 52 male and 52 female undergraduates allocated rewards to pairs of children for their performances in team and competitive situations. Males allocated rewards more equitably, especially when allocating to boys in competition, while females allocated rewards more equally. Boys, but not girls, who did more work received more rewards than their partner; and children with greater work inputs under competitive conditions received more rewards than children in team conditions. In study 2, the sex-role orientation of 60 college students was found to be related to their reward allocations. Males and females with a masculine sex-role orientation allocated rewards equitably, while individuals with a feminine sex-role orientation allocated rewards equally. Results indicated that sex and situational differences in reward allocations are affected by sex-role orientation and differential socialization practices by adults.Portions of study 1 were presented at the Midwestern Psychological Association Meeting, Chicago, May 1978. 相似文献
17.
Mirela Dubravac Claudia M. Roebers Beat Meier 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2022,40(2):287-305
Detecting an error signals the need for increased cognitive control and behavioural adjustments. Considerable development in performance monitoring and cognitive control is evidenced by lower error rates and faster response times in multi-trial executive function tasks with age. Besides these quantitative changes, we were interested in whether qualitative changes in balancing accuracy and speed contribute to developmental progression during elementary school years. We conducted two studies investigating the temporal and developmental trajectories of post-error slowing in three prominent cognitive conflict tasks (Stroop, Simon, and flanker). We instructed children (8-, 10-, and 12-year-old) and adults to respond as fast and as accurately as possible and measured their response times on four trials after correct and incorrect responses to a cognitive conflict. Results revealed that all age groups had longer response times on post-error versus post-correct trials, reflecting post-error slowing. Critically, slowing on the first post-error trial declined with age, suggesting an age-related reduction in the orienting response towards errors. This age effect diminished on subsequent trials, suggesting more fine-tuned cognitive control adjustments with age. Overall, the consistent pattern across tasks suggests an age-related change from a relatively strong orienting response to more balanced cognitive control adaptations. 相似文献
18.
Aylin Koçak Claudia Rooman Wouter Duyck Eva Derous 《Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology》2022,95(3):718-737
Age discrimination may explain lower labour market chances of older and younger job seekers. What remains underresearched, however, is how older/younger job seekers might self-select out from early recruitment procedures due to stigmatizing information in job ads. Building on theories of metastereotypes and the linguistic category model, two experimental studies investigated how personality requirements that older/younger job seekers hold negative metastereotypes about and the way in which these requirements are worded (behavioural vs. dispositional) affected their job attraction. Within-participant mediation analyses showed that as expected, job attraction was higher for older (N = 123, aged 50 years or more) and younger (N = 151, 30 years or less) job seekers when requirements were worded in a behavioural way (e.g., ‘You can be flexible’), compared with a dispositional way (e.g., ‘You are flexible’). This relation was mediated by perceptions of challenge among younger but not older job seekers. Contrary to expectations, perceptions of threat did not explain the effects of negatively metastereotyped personality requirements on job attraction. Understanding how job seekers perceive information in job ads might help recruiters to design age-sensitive recruitment policies. 相似文献
19.
20.