全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1698篇 |
免费 | 36篇 |
专业分类
1734篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 23篇 |
2020年 | 41篇 |
2019年 | 44篇 |
2018年 | 54篇 |
2017年 | 75篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 40篇 |
2014年 | 53篇 |
2013年 | 167篇 |
2012年 | 97篇 |
2011年 | 71篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 55篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 73篇 |
2006年 | 46篇 |
2005年 | 41篇 |
2004年 | 49篇 |
2003年 | 51篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 43篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 34篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1734条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
891.
892.
Cognitive styles and behavior problems of clinic boys 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Dr. Susan B. Campbell 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1974,2(4):307-312
Groups of hyperactive, learning-disabled, epileptic, and normal boys matched on age and intelligence were compared on measures of cognitive style and maternal reports of behavioral pathology. All clinical groups were more impulsive, more field dependent, and weaker automatizers than their nonclinic peers. Although the clinical groups were described by their mothers as showing more behavior problems than controls, hyperactive boys were seen as having more conduct problems, while learning-disabled boys were seen as immature. 相似文献
893.
A structured psychiatric interview designed for both clinical assessment and data collection was given to 50 pairs of well-matched pediatrie and psychiatric clinic children and their mothers. The two groups were shown to be dearly distinguished on the basis of numbers of symptoms. Behavior and school symptoms gave the sharpest distinction between the two groups. Neurotic and somatic symptoms were the poorest discriminators. The psychiatric children showed significantly more antisocial symptoms than their matched pediatrie peers. Age differences showed the expected trends with the widest divergence in the number of symptoms being in the oldest age group.This study was supported in part by NIMH Grant 05938. 相似文献
894.
895.
As part of a larger study, the authors replicated Ferreira and Winter's ( 2 ) study of family interaction and decision-making using families with a drug-abusing child. Ferreira and Winter compared normal families and families with a child diagnosed as schizophrenic, delinquent, or maladjusted. 相似文献
896.
Deafness and immediate memory for pictures: dissociations between "inner speech" and the "inner ear"? 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Born-deaf, orally trained youngsters were examined on two tasks of immediate memory for pictures of objects. The aim was to investigate the extent of speech coding for pictures in immediate memory in a developmental context. The deaf, unlike young hearing children, did not use picture-name rhyme spontaneously as a cue to recall in a paired association task. Nevertheless, they were just as sensitive as reading age-matched hearing controls to spoken word length in recalling pictures by name. This might mean that the deaf use articulatory rehearsal in some immediate memory tasks, but this leads to a paradoxical conclusion. What could "inner speech" in the deaf be for, if it fails to affect their "inner ear" by inducing rhyme sensitivity in the paired associate task? This paradox is discussed in relation to distinctions between covert and overt use of memory cues in the paired recall task and to possible sources of the word length effect in young hearing (8-9 years old) and deaf subjects. 相似文献
897.
Recent experimental studies have shown that people code time in terms of a mental timeline which typically runs from left to right or from back to front. Determining the cognitive function of this mental timeline for language processing, however, is still an unsettled issue. Whereas the studies of Ulrich and Maienborn (2010) and Ulrich et al. (2012) argue against an automatic activation of the mental timeline for the interpretation of tense and temporal adverbials at sentence level, Sell and Kaschak (2011) observe an automatic activation for the processing of past- and future-related sentences in small stories. The present paper reports the results of three experiments which examine the processing of sentences with retrospective and prospective verbs (e.g., to remember, to regret vs. to expect, to announce) in present tense, which locate a second, embedded event in the past or the future. When temporal information was task-relevant, a space–time congruency effect emerged (Experiment 1). This suggests that the mental timeline is not only linked to overtly deictic linguistic material but may also be construed in a more intricate way through the compositional construction of sentence meaning. The congruency effect disappeared, however, when temporal information was task-irrelevant (Experiments 2 and 3), suggesting that the mental timeline is not functionally involved in the cognitive processing of these especially demanding two-event sentences. The results of the present study support the conclusion that the relevant factor driving an automatic activation of the mental timeline is not the number of linguistically expressed events, but might rather be the number of sentential units. 相似文献
898.
Joshua Klayman Jack B. Soll Claudia Gonzlez-Vallejo Sema Barlas 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》1999,79(3):384
Many studies have reported that the confidence people have in their judgments exceeds their accuracy and that overconfidence increases with the difficulty of the task. However, some common analyses confound systematic psychological effects with statistical effects that are inevitable if judgments are imperfect. We present three experiments using new methods to separate systematic effects from the statistically inevitable. We still find systematic differences between confidence and accuracy, including an overall bias toward overconfidence. However, these effects vary greatly with the type of judgment. There is little general overconfidence with two-choice questions and pronounced overconfidence with subjective confidence intervals. Over- and underconfidence also vary systematically with the domain of questions asked, but not as a function of difficulty. We also find stable individual differences. Determining why some people, some domains, and some types of judgments are more prone to overconfidence will be important to understanding how confidence judgments are made. 相似文献
899.
Voelcker-Rehage C Stronge AJ Alberts JL 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2006,13(3-4):366-384
The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between force control and cognitive performance under dual-task conditions in younger (18-22 years) and older adults (65-77 years). Cognitive (n-back test) and motor performance (force-tracking) was measured independently and simultaneously. Results indicated overall age-related differences for the n-back and the force-tracking task. Age-related differences increased during dual-task conditions. While younger adults exhibited no decrease in cognitive or motor performance during dual-task conditions, older adults showed a decrease in motor and cognitive performance. Additionally, when older adults made an error in the cognitive task they tended to show greater variability in the force-tracking task. These results suggest that cognitive motor deficits are responsible for older adults' performance decrements under dual-task conditions. 相似文献
900.
Iverson P Kuhl PK Akahane-Yamada R Diesch E Tohkura Y Kettermann A Siebert C 《Cognition》2003,87(1):B47-B57
This article presents an account of how early language experience can impede the acquisition of non-native phonemes during adulthood. The hypothesis is that early language experience alters relatively low-level perceptual processing, and that these changes interfere with the formation and adaptability of higher-level linguistic representations. Supporting data are presented from an experiment that tested the perception of English /r/ and /l/ by Japanese, German, and American adults. The underlying perceptual spaces for these phonemes were mapped using multidimensional scaling and compared to native-language categorization judgments. The results demonstrate that Japanese adults are most sensitive to an acoustic cue, F2, that is irrelevant to the English /r/-/l/ categorization. German adults, in contrast, have relatively high sensitivity to more critical acoustic cues. The results show how language-specific perceptual processing can alter the relative salience of within- and between-category acoustic variation, and thereby interfere with second language acquisition. 相似文献