全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1698篇 |
免费 | 36篇 |
专业分类
1734篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 23篇 |
2020年 | 41篇 |
2019年 | 44篇 |
2018年 | 54篇 |
2017年 | 75篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 40篇 |
2014年 | 53篇 |
2013年 | 167篇 |
2012年 | 97篇 |
2011年 | 71篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 55篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 73篇 |
2006年 | 46篇 |
2005年 | 41篇 |
2004年 | 49篇 |
2003年 | 51篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 43篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 34篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1734条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
162.
Claudia J. Haferkamp 《Current Psychology》1991,10(4):227-240
Gender differences in relationship quality, conflict perceptions, reported conflict strategy use, and self-monitoring orientations
were examined in a sample of undergraduates. It was predicted that males and high self-monitoring individuals would see their
relationships as less intimate, themselves as less committed, and would endorse more frequent use of uncooperative and avoidant
strategies. Participants (n=124) completed the following: a measure of conflict perceptions; a Conflict Strategy Scale measuring frequency of Cooperative,
Uncooperative, and Denial-Avoidant strategies; and Snyder’s Self-Monitoring Scale. Compared to females, males scored higher
in self-monitoring, saw friendships as less intimate and stable, saw their conflicts as more stable, and endorsed more frequent
use of Denial-Avoidant strategies. Masculine orientations to conflict reflected increased self-presentational concerns and
perceptions that minimized the personal relevance of conflict. 相似文献
163.
164.
Boccaccini MT Epstein M Poythress N Douglas KS Campbell J Gardner G Falkenbach D 《Assessment》2007,14(4):361-374
The authors examined the relation between self-report psychopathy measures and official records of offending in four samples of justice-involved youth (total N = 447). Psychopathy measures included the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) and a modified version of the Childhood Psychopathy Scale (mCPS). Measures of offending included the total number of preadmission arrest charges for three samples (n = 392) and the total number of offenses in the year following release for two samples (n = 138). Neither measure was a strong correlate of preadmission offenses. Although mCPS scores were associated with postrelease offending in one sample, effects for the APSD were observed only when reoffending was conceptualized as a dichotomous variable, indicating a lack of robustness in this association. The findings suggest caution in the use of self-report measures of psychopathic features for decision making with respect to issues of delinquency risk among justice-involved youth. 相似文献
165.
Predicting the Early Developmental Course of Symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care were examined to test whether: attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms remain stable from 54 months through early elementary school; behavioral inhibition and attention deficits assessed at 54 months predict ADHD symptoms in elementary school, even after controlling for their temporal stability; and early behavioral inhibition and attention deficits moderate the longitudinal stability in ADHD symptoms. Data were examined using continuous and categorical measures of symptoms. Modest stability in ADHD symptoms from 54 months to third grade was found. Measures of inhibition and inattention predicted later teacher ratings uniquely, but no evidence was found for moderation. Measures of preschool behavioral inhibition also predicted “persistently at risk status” defined by elevated teacher ratings over time. Results are discussed in terms of executive and motivational facets of inhibition that may be related to early signs of ADHD. 相似文献
166.
Ian S. Penton-Voak Timothy Allen Edward R. Morrison Lisa Gralewski Neill Campbell 《Personality and individual differences》2007,43(8):2229-2236
This preliminary study investigated whether individual differences in performance on a difficult social perception task (determining the sex of shape normalized, line drawn dynamic faces) are related to sex of observer, scores on an empathy quotient and scores on a systemizing quotient. Performance in the face perception task (N = 60) was above chance, indicating that participants could judge the sex of the degraded facial stimuli from dynamic information alone. There was a trend for women to be more accurate in their judgments of target sex than men, but regression analyses indicated that EQ scores alone predicted performance on the task. This study suggests that empathy may mediate sex differences in face perception abilities, and potentially other tasks in social perception. 相似文献
167.
Pornsawan Tanchotsrinon Kakanang Maneesri W. Keith Campbell 《Journal of research in personality》2007
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between narcissism and romantic attraction in a collectivistic culture (i.e., Thailand). In Study 1, participants completed a measure of narcissism and rated their attraction to four potential romantic partners. There was an overall preference across participants for caring targets. Additionally, high narcissists were more attracted to admiring and high status targets than were low narcissists. A similar pattern of results was found in Study 2 with targets that were either high caring/low status or high status/low caring. Overall, caring in a partner was highly valued in a collectivistic culture, but narcissism still predicted attraction to targets who offered the potential for self-enhancement. These findings suggest that classically Western self-enhancement can be found in Eastern samples; however, to uncover these self-enhancement processes, researchers might need to use a personality variable such as narcissism. 相似文献
168.
José M. Oliveira Eliane Volchan Claudia D. Vargas Sônia Gleiser Isabel A. David 《Behavior research methods》2012,44(4):1115-1120
Brain event-related potentials are a useful tool for investigating visual processing and action planning. This technique requires extremely accurate synchronization of stimulus delivery with recordings. The precision of the onset time of visual stimulus delivery is a major challenge when attempting to use real, three-dimensional objects as stimuli. Here, we present an innovative device, the “box for interaction with objects” (BIO), that is designed to synchronize the presentation of objects with electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings. To reach the required resolution of stimulus-onset timing, the BIO system features an interface with reflective glass and light-emitting diodes (LEDs). When the LEDs inside the BIO are turned on, the object inside becomes visible, and a synchronizing pulse is sent to the recording systems. The BIO was tested in a motivational study that focused on visual and motor event-related potentials. EEG signals were recorded during the presentation of an emotion-laden object that could be grasped and brought close to the participant’s chest. BIO successfully synchronized the appearance of a three-dimensional object with EEG recordings, which would allow for an analysis of visual and motor event-related potentials in the same experiment. The BIO device, through a high-quality psychophysiological approach, offers a new perspective for the study of the motivational factors that drive actions toward relevant stimuli. 相似文献
169.
We investigated the effect of lexical stress on 16-month-olds' ability to form associations between labels and paths of motion. Disyllabic English nouns tend to have a strong-weak (trochaic) stress pattern, and verbs tend to have a weak-strong (iambic) pattern. We explored whether infants would use word stress information to guide word-action associations during learning. Infants heard two novel words with either verb-like iambic stress or noun-like trochaic stress. Each word was paired with a single novel object performing one of two path actions and was tested using path-switch trials. Only infants in the iambic stress condition learned the association between the novel words and the path actions. To further investigate infants' difficulty in mapping the trochaic labels to the actions, we conducted an additional study in which infants were given an object switch task using the trochaic labels. In this case, infants were able to associate the trochaic labels with the objects, providing further support that infants use lexical stress to guide label-referent associations. This study demonstrates that by 16months, English-learning infants have developed a bias to expect disyllabic action labels to have iambic stress patterns, consistent with native language stress patterns. 相似文献
170.
Guala contests the validity of strong reciprocity as a key element in shaping social behavior by contrasting evidence from experimental games to that of natural and historic data. He suggests that in order to understand the evolution of social behavior researchers should focus on natural data and weak reciprocity. We disagree with Guala's proposal to shift the focus of the study from one extreme of the spectrum (strong reciprocity) to the other extreme (weak reciprocity). We argue that the study of the evolution of social behavior must be comparative in nature, and we point out experimental evidence that shows that social behavior is not cooperation determined by a set of fixed factors. We argue for a model that sees social behavior as a dynamic interaction of genetic and environmental factors. 相似文献