首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   696篇
  免费   60篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   21篇
  2019年   22篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   25篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   83篇
  2012年   38篇
  2011年   31篇
  2010年   27篇
  2009年   18篇
  2008年   20篇
  2007年   28篇
  2006年   27篇
  2005年   18篇
  2004年   17篇
  2003年   22篇
  2002年   26篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   5篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   6篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   7篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   11篇
  1968年   6篇
  1966年   4篇
排序方式: 共有756条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
What kind of evidence will lead people to revise their moral beliefs? Moral beliefs are often strongly held convictions, and existing research has shown that morality is rooted in emotion and socialization rather than deliberative reasoning. In addition, more general issues—such as confirmation bias—further impede coherent belief revision. Here, we explored a unique means for inducing belief revision. In two experiments, participants considered a moral dilemma in which an overwhelming majority of people judged that it was inappropriate to take action to maximize utility. Their judgments contradicted a utilitarian principle they otherwise strongly endorsed. Exposure to this scenario led participants to revise their belief in the utilitarian principle, and this revision persisted over several hours. This method provides a new avenue for inducing belief revision.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
Optimal Bayesian reasoning performance has reportedly been elusive, and a variety of explanations have been suggested for this situation. In a series of experiments, it is demonstrated that these difficulties with replication can be accounted for by differences in participant-sampling methodologies. Specifically, the best performances are obtained with students from top-tier, national universities who were paid for their participation. Performance drops significantly as these conditions are altered regarding inducements (e.g., using unpaid participants) or participant source (e.g., using participants from a second-tier, regional university). Honours-programme undergraduates do better than regular undergraduates within the same university, paid participation creates superior performance, and top-tier university students do better than students from lower ranked universities. Pictorial representations (supplementing problem text) usually have a slight facilitative effect across these participant manipulations. These results indicate that studies should take account of these methodological details and focus more on relative levels of performance rather than absolute performance.  相似文献   
70.
This paper examines two topics in Japanese medical ethics: non-disclosure of medical information by Japanese physicians, and the history of human rights abuses by Japanese physicians during World War II. These contrasting issues show how culture shapes our view of ethically appropriate behavior in medicine. An understanding of cultural context reveals that certain practices, such as withholding diagnostic information from patients, may represent ethical behavior in that context. In contrast, nonconsensual human experimentation designed to harm the patient is inherently unethical irrespective of cultural context. Attempts to define moral consensus in bioethics, and to distinguish between acceptable and unacceptable variation across different cultural contexts, remain central challenges in articulating international, culturally sensitive norms in medical ethics.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号