首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   391篇
  免费   15篇
  406篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   14篇
  2017年   17篇
  2016年   21篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   48篇
  2012年   13篇
  2011年   25篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   14篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1956年   1篇
排序方式: 共有406条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
Book Reviews     
Adelmann  Pamela K.  Chamberlain  Mariam K.  Holzman  Clare G. 《Sex roles》1997,36(11-12):851-857
  相似文献   
242.
243.
244.
245.
Warr, Cook, and Wall's Job Satisfaction Scale (JSS) is a widely used measure of job satisfaction in industrial/organisational (I/O) psychology research and practice. However, the factor structure has not been adequately explored, with two‐factor and three‐factor solutions previously proposed. This study tested the factor structure of the JSS using robust analysis methods on data gathered from a convenience sample of 381 (females = 264, males = 116) Australian employees. Confirmatory factor analyses supported a hierarchical three‐factor model of job satisfaction in terms of model adequacy coefficients; however, the three factors were highly correlated, thereby rendering a multifactorial approach to the JSS untenable. The results support the continued use of an overall score of job satisfaction when using this measure in I/O psychology research and practice. Further testing of the structure is recommended within a range of employment sectors, as the assumed multifactorial structure of the JSS common in the literature was not supported by the current study.  相似文献   
246.
247.
248.
The expertise paradigm was used in two studies to examine decision-making by rugby referees. Videoclips were used to assess infraction detection and knowledge base, or sources of information used. In Study 1, referees of high and low experience were compared, and in Study 2 referees and players were compared. Analysis by signal detection used a framework to classify types of information. Study 1 yielded no differences in detection of infractions as a function of experience, however, referees of high experience used significantly more sources of information than the group with low experience across all categories of information. In Study 2, there were no significant differences between referees and players, with the exception that referees displayed greater use of episodic memory information in decision-making.  相似文献   
249.
The present study examined sleep architecture as a function of handedness in a population of undergraduate college women using a home sleep monitor. Compared to strongly handed individuals, participants with a tendency toward mixed-handedness had a shorter sleep latency and spent a greater percentage of their sleep period asleep and less awake. Increasing mixed-handedness was also associated with increased NREM; strong-handedness was associated with increased REM. Results are placed in a neurophysiological framework wherein corpus callosum mediated differences in interhemispheric interaction during Wake, REM, and NREM on the one hand, and individual differences in corpus callosum morphology and hemispheric communication as a function of handedness on the other, interact to result in handedness differences in sleep architecture.  相似文献   
250.
Children ages 6, 8 and 10 (N = 66) distributed resources to a boy and a girl when the proportion of feminine and masculine resources varied, when the preferences of the boy and the girl varied, and when the setting was public or private. Children used gender norms to sort the resources but they primarily sorted the resources equally, even when it required violating gender norms. When there was an atypical preference, participants provided more atypical resources to the children. However, when the setting was public, they provided fewer atypical resources. Overall the results indicate that gender norms are especially strong in public settings but explicit information about preferences can lead children to be flexible about gender norms.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号