全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1144篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
1154篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 43篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 46篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 50篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 58篇 |
2011年 | 62篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 49篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 51篇 |
2006年 | 46篇 |
2005年 | 29篇 |
2004年 | 40篇 |
2003年 | 28篇 |
2002年 | 30篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1154条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
Butler KM McDaniel MA Dornburg CC Price AL Roediger HL 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2004,11(5):921-925
The relationship of neuropsychological measures of frontal lobe function to age differences in false recall was assessed using the Deese/Roediger-McDermott associative false memory paradigm (Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995). As other studies have found, older adults were less likely to correctly recall studied items and more likely to falsely recall highly related but nonpresented items than were younger adults. When older adults were divided based on a composite measure of frontal lobe functioning, this age difference was found only for low frontal lobe functioning individuals. High frontal lobe functioning older adults and young adults had equivalent levels of false recall, as well as equivalent levels of veridical recall. These results suggest that age differences in memory may be due to declines in frontal lobe function. More important, our findings indicate that declines in veridical recall and increases in false recall are not an inevitable consequence of aging. 相似文献
902.
Building on an existing latent variable analysis of executive function (EF) in children (N = 191, 57% boys and 43% girls) making the transition to school (Hughes et al. (2010), Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 35, pp. 20-36), the current study both documented average developmental improvements from 4 to 6 years of age and examined individual differences in EF growth in relation to latent factors for two sets of child outcome measures at 6 years: (a) first-grade teachers’ ratings of emotional symptoms, hyperactivity, and conduct/peer problems and (b) children’s self-perceived academic and social competencies. With effects of concurrent verbal ability and EF controlled, variation in EF slopes across the transition to school predicted variation in latent constructs for (a) all four problem behavior subscales and (b) children’s self-reported academic (but not social) competence. These findings underscore the clinical and educational significance of early individual differences in EF and highlight the value of adopting a developmental perspective. 相似文献
903.
904.
Price MA Butow PN Lo SK Wilson J;Kathleen Cuningham Consortium for Research into Familial Breast Cancer 《Journal of genetic counseling》2007,16(5):635-644
Some women at increased familial risk of breast cancer experience elevated levels of cancer-specific worry, which can possibly
act as a barrier to screening, and may be a significant factor in decisions regarding risk-reducing surgery. The aim of this
study was to comprehensively examine predictors of cancer-specific worry in high risk women and to test a model which proposes
that perceived breast cancer risk mediates the impact of other factors on worry. 1,437 unaffected women from high risk breast
cancer families completed questionnaires and interviews. Path analysis was used to test the model of potential predictors
of cancer worry, including familial, personal and psychological variables, mediated via perceived cancer risk. Levels of cancer-specific
worry were generally low despite an average perceived risk of 50.3%. The goodness-of-fit of the proposed model was poor, explaining
only 9% of the variance for perceived risk and 10% of the variance for cancer specific worry. An alternative model of a direct
relationship between all of the predictor variables and cancer worry, explained 24% of the variation in cancer worry. General
anxiety, perceived risk, the stressful impact of recent cancer related events, a relative risk greater than 10, being closer
in age to the youngest breast cancer diagnosis in family, and knowledge of personal mutation status, all independently contributed
to cancer worry. Addressing general affective responses, experiences of recent cancer related events, in addition to education
about personal risk, should be considered in counselling women with elevated cancer worry. Risk perception appears to act
independently of other factors in its formulation and impact on cancer worry. Further research on the way in which women come
to perceive their risk is indicated.
The kConFab Psychosocial Group are (in alphabetical order of institution): Brain and Mind Institute, University of Sydney,
Australia (I Hickie) Department of Haematology and Medical Oncology, Peter MacCallum Cancer Centre, Melbourne, Australia (K-A
Phillips) Department of Medical Oncology, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, Australia (B Bennett, B Meiser, K Tucker) Department
of Oncology, St Vincent’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (S-A MaLachlan) Department of Psychological Medicine, Royal North
Shore Hospital, St Leonards, Australia (C Tennant); Medical Psychology Research Unit, University of Sydney, Australia (P Butow,
M Price). 相似文献
905.
John E. Puddifoot Claire A. Cooke 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》2002,12(4):256-270
The representation of handguns by a sample of university students (n = 96) from one of the southern states of the US, was contrasted with that of a sample of university students in the UK (n = 100), employing ANOVA and Hierarchical Cluster Analysis. Exploring the structure of response, the findings showed little evidence for a common cross‐locational representation of handguns for the US and UK samples taken together. There was support for the existence of distinctive locational representations, some support for gender‐specific representations, and strong support for representations of handguns according to particular combinations of locality and gender. Representations of handguns by US males from the southern states were the most distinctive, complex and integrated in structure, bringing into question simplistic assumptions concerning the nature of an alleged ‘southern sub‐culture of violence’ or of a more general ‘gun culture’. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
906.
Four hundred and thirty-four children enrolled in school years 5 and 6 in the United Kingdom were administered measures of
trust beliefs in peers/best friends and psychosocial functioning (internalized maladjustment, self-perceived social acceptance,
social preference, and social exclusion) across an 8-month period (mean age = 9 years − 9 months at Time 1). The relation
between children's trust beliefs in peers or trust beliefs within best friend dyads and measures of psychosocial functioning
conformed to a quadratic pattern. Compared to children in the middle range of trust beliefs, children with very low trust
beliefs and those with very high trust beliefs in peers and/or within best friend dyads displayed higher internalized maladjustment,
lower self-perceived social acceptance, higher social exclusion, and lower social preference. The relation between the trust
beliefs and internalized maladjustment was asymmetrical, with children who held very low trust beliefs being comparatively
more disadvantaged. 相似文献
907.
908.
In 5 experiments, college students exhibited a group size effect on risk judgments. As the number of individuals in a target group increased, so did participants' judgments of the risk of the average member of the group for a variety of negative life events. This happened regardless of whether the stimuli consisted of photographs of real peers or stick-figure representations of peers. As a result, the degree to which participants exhibited comparative optimism (i.e., judged themselves to be at lower risk than their peers) also increased as the size of the comparison group increased. These results suggest that the typical comparative optimism effect reported so often in the literature might be, at least in part, a group size effect. Additional results include a group size effect on judgments of the likelihood that the average group member will experience positive and neutral events and a group size effect on perceptual judgments of the heights of stick figures. These latter results, in particular, support the existence of a simple, general cognitive mechanism that integrates stimulus numerosity into quantitative judgments about that stimulus. 相似文献
909.
Few studies have investigated how physical and social facial cues are integrated in the formation of face preferences. Here we show that expression differentially qualifies the strength of attractiveness preferences for faces with direct and averted gaze. For judgments of faces with direct gaze, attractiveness preferences were stronger for smiling faces than for faces with neutral expressions. By contrast, for judgments of faces with averted gaze, attractiveness preferences were stronger for faces with neutral expressions than for smiling faces. Because expressions can differ in meaning depending on whether they are directed toward or away from oneself, it is only by integrating gaze direction, facial expression, and physical attractiveness that one can unambiguously identify the most attractive individuals who are likely to reciprocate one's own social interest. 相似文献
910.