全文获取类型
收费全文 | 54194篇 |
免费 | 645篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
54841篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 399篇 |
2019年 | 504篇 |
2018年 | 3978篇 |
2017年 | 3360篇 |
2016年 | 2799篇 |
2015年 | 685篇 |
2014年 | 638篇 |
2013年 | 2830篇 |
2012年 | 1575篇 |
2011年 | 3384篇 |
2010年 | 3120篇 |
2009年 | 2085篇 |
2008年 | 2607篇 |
2007年 | 2965篇 |
2006年 | 882篇 |
2005年 | 901篇 |
2004年 | 802篇 |
2003年 | 799篇 |
2002年 | 681篇 |
2001年 | 1244篇 |
2000年 | 1243篇 |
1999年 | 871篇 |
1998年 | 407篇 |
1997年 | 328篇 |
1996年 | 306篇 |
1994年 | 317篇 |
1992年 | 725篇 |
1991年 | 709篇 |
1990年 | 673篇 |
1989年 | 618篇 |
1988年 | 593篇 |
1987年 | 586篇 |
1986年 | 551篇 |
1985年 | 618篇 |
1984年 | 478篇 |
1983年 | 372篇 |
1979年 | 468篇 |
1978年 | 343篇 |
1977年 | 329篇 |
1976年 | 305篇 |
1975年 | 416篇 |
1974年 | 422篇 |
1973年 | 477篇 |
1972年 | 388篇 |
1971年 | 325篇 |
1970年 | 308篇 |
1969年 | 331篇 |
1968年 | 386篇 |
1967年 | 338篇 |
1966年 | 392篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
First-grade children were given either empathy instructions (in which they were told to imagine themselves in the target person's place) or neutral instructions (in which they were told to listen to what the target person did). For half the subjects, the target person was of the same sex; for the other half, the target person was of the opposite sex. Subjects were then given a chance to anonymously donate money to the target child, who was presented as being in need. Liking for the target child, subject's perceptions of the target child's emotional state, and subjects' report of their own emotional states were also assessed. Empathic instructions increased donating behavior for males, but not for females. Accuracy of perception of the target person's state was generally high. Self-reports of emotional states congruent with that of the target tended to be associated with increased donating behavior only for males, but these self-reports were greater for females than males, and greater for both sexes with same-sex target persons. Liking also was enhanced by gender similarity. These results are discussed in terms of a proposed relationship among empathy, prosocial behavior, sex, and age.We wish to express our gratitude to the staff and students of Wannamaker and North Fairview Elementary Schools of Topeka, Kansas, for their assistance in carrying out this research, and also thank C. Daniel Batson for his helpful suggestions and comments. 相似文献
242.
243.
244.
Joanne Benjamin-Bauman Maxin L. Reiss Jon S. Bailey 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1984,17(3):295-301
We examined the effect of reducing the interval between a patient's call for an appointment and the appointment itself. In Experiment 1, patients calling a family planning unit of a public health department were assigned appointments within either 1 or 3 weeks of their call. Data on patient “shows” and “no-shows” were recorded weekly for 6 weeks. Show rates for those in the 1-week appointment group were significantly higher than those in the 3-week group. In Experiment 2, patients were assigned to appointment dates either the next operating clinic day (next-day group) or 2 weeks from the call date (2-week group). Show rates for those in the next-day group were significantly better than show rates for patients in the 2-week group. Clinic productivity, time spent with patients, and consumer satisfaction were also assessed. Implications for appointment scheduling are discussed. 相似文献
245.
S. Graham Kosch Ph.D. Charles A. Reiner Ph.D. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1984,14(2):145-157
This project, utilizing a pretest and posttest design, studied interactions between co-therapists and the relationships of their interactions to client outcome. Six co-therapist pairs at a counseling center saw two clients in multiple treatments yielding twelve cases. The study investigated co-therapist relationships in regard to: (a) their level of mutuality or caring for their partners, (b) their ratings of the quality of their relationships, and (c) their agreement as to perceptions and behavior during sessions. Also investigated were: the relation of co-therapist interaction to client outcome and the personal growth of the therapists over the course of therapy. Results supported success claims of the literature concerning co-therapy. 相似文献
246.
247.
A brief version of the Multiscore Depression Inventory was evaluated. Good coefficient alpha and test-retest reliabilities were obtained for the total score and the subscales. Two factor analyses were reported which appear to justify the use of a multiscore approach for at least 8 of the 9 subscales: helplessness, energy level, guilt, social introversion, cognitive difficulty, irritability, pessimism, and low self-esteem. Sad mood apparently overlapped with energy level on one analysis (replicating findings with the 118-item Multiscore Depression Inventory), and with self-esteem on the second analysis. Normative data are provided on diverse samples totaling 849 subjects. 相似文献
248.
Attributional models of depression suggest that depression is associated with internal, stable, and global causal explanations of events. However, the models do not specify whether the depressive attributional process involves the selection of certain causes, the assignment of dimensional meanings to those causes, or both. A distinction between these two processes was suggested by the findings that the choice of causes does not reliably indicate the perceived dimensions and that there is poor convergence between existing methods that assess causes and those that assess dimensions. When causes and dimensions were each measured in a manner that avoided confounding by the other, each made a unique contribution to the prediction of depression scores. Cause ratings alone were significantly associated with depression scores, but the optimal prediction of depression included both causes and dimensions. The implications of the distinction between these two processes for experimental manipulations of attributions and for reattribution training programs are discussed. 相似文献
249.
P M Greenfield E S Savage-Rumbaugh 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1984,98(2):201-218
Analysis of two chimpanzees' conversations with their teacher during a tool-use training task demonstrated that chimps use lexigrams, a humanly devised visual symbol system, selectively to encode perceived variability; that is, they generally used their symbols to differentiate alternative possibilities or to represent change or novelty in a situation. In contrast, they tended to leave unsaid what was unchanging, repetitive, or the unique possibility in a situation. Perceived variability influenced not only which symbols were selected but also utterance length: A single dimension of variability in a situation leads to single-lexigram utterances; multiple dimensions are associated with multi-lexigram utterances. This pattern of results indicates that the absence of formal grammatical structure in chimp language does not imply that utterances beyond one word in length are either rote strings or imitations. The chimps' tendency to mention the variable while leaving the constant or redundant unsaid is, moreover, strong support for the position that their use of a humanly devised symbol system is more than a series of conditioned responses. 相似文献
250.