首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7380篇
  免费   1550篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2023年   54篇
  2022年   40篇
  2021年   118篇
  2020年   181篇
  2019年   467篇
  2018年   392篇
  2017年   508篇
  2016年   517篇
  2015年   409篇
  2014年   398篇
  2013年   988篇
  2012年   451篇
  2011年   406篇
  2010年   359篇
  2009年   325篇
  2008年   331篇
  2007年   285篇
  2006年   266篇
  2005年   242篇
  2004年   204篇
  2003年   219篇
  2002年   221篇
  2001年   168篇
  2000年   123篇
  1999年   88篇
  1998年   61篇
  1997年   54篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   57篇
  1994年   54篇
  1993年   39篇
  1992年   55篇
  1991年   43篇
  1990年   39篇
  1989年   37篇
  1988年   40篇
  1987年   40篇
  1986年   41篇
  1985年   41篇
  1984年   36篇
  1983年   40篇
  1982年   41篇
  1981年   30篇
  1980年   37篇
  1979年   41篇
  1978年   46篇
  1977年   35篇
  1976年   37篇
  1975年   30篇
  1974年   31篇
排序方式: 共有8941条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
Communicating with multiple addressees poses a problem for speakers: Each addressee necessarily comes to the conversation with a different perspective—different knowledge, different beliefs, and a distinct physical context. Despite the ubiquity of multiparty conversation in everyday life, little is known about the processes by which speakers design language in multiparty conversation. While prior evidence demonstrates that speakers design utterances to accommodate addressee knowledge in multiparty conversation, it is unknown if and how speakers encode and combine different types of perspective information. Here we test whether speakers encode the perspective of multiple addressees, and then simultaneously consider their knowledge and physical context during referential design in a three‐party conversation. Analyses of referential form—expression length, disfluency, and elaboration rate—in an interactive multiparty conversation demonstrate that speakers do take into consideration both addressee knowledge and physical context when designing utterances, consistent with a knowledge‐scene integration view. These findings point to an audience design process that takes as input multiple types of representations about the perspectives of multiple addressees, and that bases the informational content of the to‐be‐designed utterance on a combination of the perspectives of the intended addressees.  相似文献   
942.
Personality traits and risk perceptions were examined as predictors of changes in smoking behavior. Participants (N = 697) were part of a randomized controlled trial of interventions to reduce exposure to the combined hazard of radon and cigarette smoke. Participants with higher perceived risk at baseline for the combination of smoking and radon were more likely to have a more restrictive household smoking ban in place at 12-month follow-up (p < .05). Risk perceptions also predicted reductions in the total number of cigarettes smoked in the home for participants in the video intervention who had high or moderate levels of extraversion (p < .01). Greater perceived risk predicted whether highly or moderately conscientious women quit smoking (p < .05). The moderating effects of personality traits should be considered when evaluating risk-reduction interventions.  相似文献   
943.
944.
945.
946.
947.
948.
Research has indicated that individuals possessing psychopathic traits exhibit a deficit in the processing of emotional stimuli. Lexical decision task studies found that psychopathic individuals do not demonstrate affective facilitation in processing emotional words relative to nonpsychopathic individuals. However, these investigations have not examined processing of discrete affective categories and their relation to the callous/unemotional (F1) and impulse control/antisocial (F2) factors of psychopathy. Sixty undergraduate men completed a self-report measure of psychopathy traits and a lexical decision task assessing response latencies to anger, sadness, fear, and happiness words. Results reflected an association between F2 and a heightened experience of anger, whereas F1 was associated with a diminished experience of sadness. Findings are discussed in terms of the relation to existing research using alternative methods of processing affect.  相似文献   
949.
950.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号